Skip to main content
Log in

“Not Thinking” Helps Reasoning

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite previous research suggesting that participants’ negative and positive emotions can impair and facilitate reasoning performance, a recent study proposes that the emotional content of the mood induction materials may not be crucial in eliciting these phenomena—deontic selection task performance is as poor when these materials are neutral as when they are negative (Perham and Oaksford 2006). We extend this finding to syllogistic reasoning performance. Participants in the mood induction conditions (negative and neutral) verbally described their experiences in relation to twenty negative or neutral words whereas participants in the control condition received no such mood induction. Subsequent syllogistic reasoning performance was significantly poorer for both mood induction conditions yet only those in the negative mood induction condition showed a significant increase in anxiety. Results imply that the key mechanism involved in the impairment derives from the production of irrelevant thoughts and that these need not be linked to a positive or negative mood, thus “not thinking” may actually help the reasoning process.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Appleby, P., & (Executive Producer). (1989). Wildlife on one. Bristol: BBC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24(4), 561–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report C-1. Gainesville: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Channon, S., & Baker, J. (1994). Reasoning strategies in depression—effects of depressed mood on a syllogism task. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(5), 707–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 87–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1998). Working memory capacity in high trait anxious individuals and repressors. Cognition and Emotion, 12(5), 697–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior (pp. 25–43). Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1979). Anxiety, learning, and memory: A reconceptualization. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 363–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, D. M., & Tremblay, S. (2000). Interference in memory by process or content? A reply to Neath (2000). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 550–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, D. M., Farrand, P., Stuart, G., & Morris, N. (1995). Functional equivalence of verbal and spatial information in serial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 1008–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manktelow, K. I., & Over, D. E. (1991). Social roles and utilities in reasoning with deontic conditionals. Cognition, 39, 85–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melton, R. J. (1995). The role of positive affect in syllogism performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 788–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Millar, N., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening facial expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 19, 695–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2007). Bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B., & Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Mood, reasoning, and central executive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(2), 476–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perham, N., & Oaksford, M. (2005). Deontic reasoning with emotional content: Evolutionary psychology or decision theory? Cognitive Science, 29, 681–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perham, N. & Oaksford, M. (2006). Experienced and anticipated emotion in deontic reasoning. Proceedings of the 28th cognitive science annual conference, Vancouver, Canada.

  • Perham, N., Banbury, S., & Jones, D. M. (2005). Auditory distraction impairs analytical reasoning performance. In M. Katsikitis (Ed.), Past reflections, future directions: Proceedings of the 40th Australian psychological society annual conference (pp. 238–242). Melbourne: APS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perham, N., Banbury, S., & Jones, D. M. (2007). Reduction in auditory distraction by retrieval strategy. Memory, 15, 465–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. (2002). Constraints on statistical learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, G., de Jong, P. J., & Mayer, B. (2000). If you suffer from a headache, then you have a brain tumour: Domain-specific reasoning ‘bias’ and hypochondriasis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 763–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1970). The State trait anxiety inventory (STAI) test manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Chan, S., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Eade, J., & Healy, H. (2000). Retrieval of autobiographical memories: The mechanisms and consequences of truncated search. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 351–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., & Soulsby, J. (2000). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory in formerly depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 150–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nick Perham.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Perham, N., Rosser, J. “Not Thinking” Helps Reasoning. Curr Psychol 31, 160–167 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-012-9140-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-012-9140-7

Keywords

Navigation