Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The development of reading comprehension skills in children learning English as a second language

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reading comprehension is a multi-dimensional process that includes the reader, the text, and factors associated with the activity of reading. Most research and theories of comprehension are based primarily on research conducted with monolingual English speakers (L1). The present study was designed to investigate the cognitive and linguistic factors that have an influence on reading comprehension in English-as-a-Second-Language (ESL) speakers. The cognitive aspects of reading comprehension among L1 speakers and ESL speakers in the seventh grade were investigated. The performance of both groups was compared and the role of some relevant processes, including word reading, word reading fluency, phonological awareness, working memory, and morphological and syntactic awareness were assessed. Within this sample, three groups were examined: (1) children with poor comprehension (PC) in the absence of word reading difficulties (2) children with poor word reading and poor comprehension (poor readers, PR) (3) and children with both good word reading and comprehension abilities (good comprehenders, GC). The results demonstrated that a variety of cognitive processes, such as working memory and phonological, syntactic, and morphological awareness are important for reading comprehension and compromised in poor comprehenders. The GC group performed better than the PC group on all of the cognitive measures, indicating that comprehension depends on a variety of phonological, memory and linguistic processes and that adequate word recognition skill are important for reading comprehension. The prevalence of the ESL and L1 students was similar across the three reading groups. The ESL and L1 students demonstrated similar performance, indicating that the skills underlying reading comprehension are similar in the ESL and L1 students. This study demonstrated that ESL students are capable of developing word reading and reading comprehension skills that are as strong as those of their L1 peers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarts, R., & Verhoeven, L. (1999). Literacy attainment in a second language submersion context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 377–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnbak, E., & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the reading and spelling skills of young dyslexics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 44, 229–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 302, 311–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (2003). Oral comprehension sets the ceiling on reading comprehension. American Educator, 27, 23–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blachman, B. (2000). Phonological awareness. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 483–502). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 683–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 428–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1986). Language proficiency and language achievement. In J. Cummins & M. Swain (Eds.), Bilingualism in education (pp. 138–161). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., & Siegel, L. S. (2004). Early literacy instruction, SES, and reading development in English language learners and children with English as a first language. Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice, 19, 202–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., Siegel, L. S., & Maggi, S. (2004). Literacy instruction, SES and word reading achievement in English language learners and children with English as a first language: A longitudinal study. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19, 202–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Da Fontoura, H. A., & Siegel, L. S. (1995). Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 139–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M. V., & Verhoeven, L. (1998). Background knowledge, linguistic complexity, and second-language reading comprehension. Journal of Literacy Research, 30, 253–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M. V., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 78–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C (1998). Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In J. L Metsala & L. C Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 3–40). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • García, G. E. (1991). Factors influencing the English reading test performance of Spanish-speaking Hispanic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 371–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Understanding differences in word recognition skills of ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 123–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gholamain, M., & Geva, E. (1999). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in English and Persian. Language Learning, 49, 183–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A. (2002). Language and reading skills in bilingual Spanish-English speakers. Topics in Language Disorders, 23, 42–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 563–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for measuring and understanding individual differences in the component processes of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kame’enui, E. J., & Simmons, D. C. (2001). The DNA of reading fluency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karlsen, B., & Gardner, E. (1994). Stanford diagnostic reading test. San Francisco: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). Investigating cognitive and linguistic abilities that influence the reading comprehension skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Reading and Writing, 19, 99–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Rupp, A. A., & Siegel, L. S. (2007). Growth in reading skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds: Findings from a 5-Year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 821–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1005–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limbos, L. M., & Geva, E. (2001). Accuracy of teacher assessments of second-language students at risk for reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 136–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, P., & Siegel, L. S. (2005). A comparison of the cognitive processes underlying reading comprehension in native English and ESL speakers. Written Language and Literacy, 8, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. A. (1984). Longitudinal prediction and prevention of early reading difficulty. Annals of Dyslexia, 34, 117–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letters knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51, 187–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1997). Assessing reading difficulties: The validity and utility of current measures of reading skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 359–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction Vol NIH Pub. No 00–4754. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development: Bethesda, Maryland.

  • North Vancouver School District No. 44. (2001). Firm foundations: Early literacy teaching and training. North Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: North Vancouver School District No. 44.

    Google Scholar 

  • North Vancouver School District No. 44. (2004). Reading 44 North Vancouver School District: A core reading framework. North Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: North Vancouver School District No. 44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J. V. (1994). Individual differences in children’s text comprehension. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 821–848). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1988). Verbal efficiency in reading ability. In M. Daneman, G. E. MacKinnon, & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 109–143). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merryl-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical bases of comprehension skill. In D. Gorfien (Ed.), On the consequences of meaning selection (pp. 67–86). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Marron, M. A., & Foltz, P. W. (1996). Sources of comprehension failure: Theoretical perspectives and case studies. In C. Cornoldi & J. V. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and remediation (pp. 137–165). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perie, M., Grigg, W., & Donahue, P. (2005). The nation’s report card: Reading 2005 (NCES 2006–451). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

  • RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. Washington, DC: RAND Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge (pp. 99–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D. (1989). How problems of comprehension are related to difficulties decoding. In D. Shankweiler & I. Y. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology and reading disabilities: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 35–68). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1993). The development of reading. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 63–97). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phonological processing deficits as the basis of dyslexia: Implications for remediation. In M. J. Riddoch & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation (pp. 379–400). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (2008). Morphological awareness skills of English language learners and children with dyslexia. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 15–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989a). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60, 973–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989b). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: The influence of definitional variables. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1, 257–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences in individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable difference model of reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Howell, M. (2001). Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictor of children’s reading performance at different ages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 720–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., Saez, L., & Gerber, M. (2006). Growth in literacy and cognition in bilingual children at risk or not at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 247–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit. Issues in Education, 7, 1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Wright, A. D. (1987). Syntactic awareness and reading acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. (1990). Acquisition of reading in a second language. Reading Research Quarterly, 25, 90–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. T. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 313–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processes. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The wide range achievement test—3. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D., & Ryan, E. B. (1981). Differential utilization of syntactic and semantic information by skilled and less skilled readers in the intermediate grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 607–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D. M., & Ryan, E. B. (1986). The development of grammatical sensitivity and its relation to early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 253–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. V. (1991). Children’s problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N. M., Oakhill, J. V., & Parkin, A. J. (1989). Working memory, comprehension skill and the resolution of text anomaly. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 351–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to L.S. Siegel. The authors would like to thank the children that participated in this study, the teachers, and administrators for their cooperation and support. This study was part of a thesis of the first author. We would like to thank the thesis committee, Dr. Laurie Ford and Professor Bruno Zumbo, for their valuable support and comments during the writing of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Orly Lipka.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Planet filk

The silver spaceship, Starview, glided to a gentle stop on the planet Filk. Just as the landing wheels touched down, Pat and Kim, the passengers, both age 12, felt the spaceship bump.

They peered out the window. Pat screamed, “Look at the lake, look at the lake! It’s purple!”

Kim said, “That is because it is made out of grape juice.”

“You’re kidding me. Lakes are not made out of grape juice.”

“Yes, they are. They are on Filk.”

“Really? Are you fooling me?”

“They are made out of grape juice. Let’s go taste what is in the lake. Then you’ll believe me.”

While strolling along the path to the lake, they heard a rustling in the bushes. “What’s that, Pat?”. Pat looked scared, “I don’t know”. All of a sudden a big bear with blue fur appeared in front of a bush just a short distance away. The bear stared at them. What should they do? They tried to remember what to do when you meet a bear. Run? Climb a tree? Make noise?

“What should we do?” asked Kim.

Just then the bear started to come toward them. They were scared. Suddenly, the bear turned and ran away into the bushes. “Wow, that was close. I hope we don’t meet any more bears.”

They dashed to the lake and scooped up the purple liquid with their hands. “Yum! This tastes good, Pat. I wish the lakes on Earth were made of grape juice.”

In the distance they spotted a large tree with different coloured flowers on it: red, green, yellow, white, orange, brown. The flowers were round and very smooth. What strange looking flowers, they thought. When they reached the tree they saw they were not flowers; they were Smarties. Pat reached up and grabbed one. So did Kim. The Smarties tasted good. In the distance they saw a person. It must be someone who lives on Filk. People who live on Filk are called Filkians. Pat whispered, “Look! The Filkian has three heads! Wow, they must be very smart.”

“Having three big brains does not always mean that you are smart. Elephants have big brains and they’re not very smart”, said Kim.

“The Filkian has no ears!” Kim exclaimed. Pat pointed to the large antenna on each of the Filkian’s three heads. “Maybe that’s what they use to hear.”

“I bet that you’re right.”

The Filkian had only one mouth in the middle of his head.

“Ellohay, Earth eoplepay.” A Filkian walked slowly toward them.

“What did the Filkian say?” asked Pat.

“I don’t understand.” Kim replied. “It sounds like Martian. I guess he’s speaking Filkian.”

Pat and Kim spoke to the Filkian, “We don’t understand. We speak English. Do you speak English?”

“Onay. I ouldway ikelay ota earnlay. Illway ouya eachtay emay?”

“Okay. When we meet people we say ‘hello’.”

Pat and Kim both said, “Hello.”

Pointing and smiling, Pat said, “My name is Pat.” Pointing to Kim, Pat said, “My friend’s name is Kim.” Pat pointed to the Filkian. “What is your name?”

“89213.”

“Do you have numbers instead of names?”

“Esyay.”

The Filkian waved and said “Oodgay ayday.”

Pat and Kim said “Goodbye.”

They looked ahead of them.

Large and beautiful mountains rose majestically in the distance. They were brown with white tops.

“Let’s go visit the mountains.”

“How will we get there?”

Pat looked ahead and saw a sign ‘TRAIN TO THE MOUNTAINS. 200 KM FROM HERE TO THE MOUNTAINS. NEXT TRAIN 12 NOON. TRAIN ARRIVES 1400 (2 PM). RETURN TRAIN AT 1600 (4PM). FARE: FREE ONLY FOR EARTH PEOPLE. FARE FOR FILKIANS: 27 PABBERS.

“Let’s go!”

“Do we have time?”

“Yes. The spaceship leaves at 1900 (7 pm). We can catch the train at 1600 and be at the spaceship in plenty of time.”

Pat and Kim took the train to the mountains. They threw snowballs at each other on the top of the mountains. They had a good time and returned to the spaceship.

Questions

  1. 1.

    If Kim and Pat saw a Filkian wearing a fur coat made of bearskin, the coat would be

    1. a.

      Brown

    2. b.

      Blue

    3. c.

      White

    4. d.

      Black

  2. 2.

    Which of the following could be a word in Filkian language?

    1. a.

      Latcay

    2. b.

      Otnak

    3. c.

      Onpat

    4. d.

      Ogday

  3. 3.

    Majestic means

    1. a.

      Very big and beautiful

    2. b.

      Very little and beautiful

    3. c.

      Average size and beautiful

    4. d.

      No way to know

  4. 4.

    How fast does the train to the mountains travel?

    1. a.

      200 km per hour

    2. b.

      100 km per hour

    3. c.

      50 km per hour

    4. d.

      150 km per hour

  5. 5.

    Will Pat and Kim return to the spaceship on time?

    1. a.

      Yes

    2. b.

      Maybe

    3. c.

      No

    4. d.

      If they hurry

  6. 6.

    If Pat and Kim were thirsty and decided to drink from the lake, what would they drink?

    1. a.

      Water

    2. b.

      Grape juice

    3. c.

      Milk

    4. d.

      Hot chocolate

  7. 7.

    Who said, “Yes they are on Filk.”

    1. a.

      Kim

    2. b.

      Pat

    3. c.

      Neither Kim nor Pat

    4. d.

      Both Kim and Pat

  8. 8.

    Were Pat and Kim boys or girls?

    1. a.

      They were both boys.

    2. b.

      Pat was a girl and Kim was a boy.

    3. c.

      Kim was a girl and Pat was a boy.

    4. d.

      The story does not tell us.

  9. 9.

    What time will the train return from the mountains?

    1. a.

      1600

    2. b.

      1800

    3. c.

      1900

    4. d.

      1400

  10. 10.

    The Filkian money is called

    1. a.

      Filkers

    2. b.

      Pabbers

    3. c.

      Dollars

    4. d.

      Cents

Greb

The spaceship, Starview, drifted silently down from the clouds to the Planet Greb. The passengers, Leslie and Cory, age 11 years old, peered out the large round window and saw a grassy meadow.

“Look at the turquoise coloured leaves on the trees!” shouted Leslie.

“All the trees have turquoise leaves, Leslie,” Cory said.

They started off down the path. A large creature stepped in front of them.

“Greb to Welcome! Stay your enjoy you hope I.” The creature grinned at them with a mouthful of teeth.

“What did the creature say?”

“I don’t know; it must speak the Grebian language.”

The creature yelled at them: “Me understand you don’t?”

“No, I’m sorry; we only speak English.”

“You understand don’t I.” the creature said and waved goodbye.

“Look at its hand! It has 12 fingers on it, neatly arranged in three rows of four fingers!” The other hand has 12 also, in three rows.

“Don’t stare. He probably thinks that we are funny looking.”

Leslie and Cory ran off to the spaceship.

Questions

  1. 1.

    Leslie kicked a pile of leaves. What colour were the leaves?

    1. a.

      Green

    2. b.

      Red

    3. c.

      Yellow

    4. d.

      Turquoise

  2. 2.

    Which one of these sentences should be correct in the Grebian language?

    1. a.

      Strange and scary very is large planet this.

    2. b.

      Strange and scary very is planet large this.

    3. c.

      Strange and scary planet is very large.

    4. d.

      Strange and scary larger very this planet.

  3. 3.

    What is one advantage of having 24 fingers, 12 on each hand, arranged in three rows?

    1. a.

      You could play the piano very easily.

    2. b.

      You could type on a computer very easily.

    3. c.

      You could knit more easily than someone with only 5 fingers on each hand.

    4. d.

      You could pick your nose more easily than someone with 5 fingers on each hand.

  4. 4.

    Which is correct?

    1. a.

      Cory and Leslie are both boys.

    2. b.

      Cory is a boy and Leslie is a girl.

    3. c.

      Cory and Leslie are both girls.

    4. d.

      There is not enough information to judge from the story.

Appendix 2

Oral cloze—grade 7

Instructions: I will read something to you and there will be a word missing. Where the word is missing, I will say “blank.” I want you to think of a word that would sound right in the blank. For example, I might say, “The moon shines bright in the ‘blank’” (pause and repeat) and I want you to say “sky” or “night.” “The moon shines bright in the sky.” O.k. let’s try another one. I’ll say, “The children ‘blank’ with the toys.” (pause and repeat). What’s the word missing? (If the child fails to respond, say “How about play? The children play with the toys.”) Let’s try another one. “The little puppy wags its ___” (pause and repeat). “Good!”

  1. 1.

    The ___ little pigs ate corn.

  2. 2.

    Fred put the big turkey ___ the oven.

  3. 3.

    The ___ put his dairy cows in the barn.

  4. 4.

    Jane ___ her sister ran up the hill.

  5. 5.

    It was a sunny day with a pretty ___ sky.

  6. 6.

    Betty ___ a hole with her shovel.

  7. 7.

    we should have____that yesterday.

  8. 8.

    With a piece ___ chalk, he sketched her face.

  9. 9.

    The girl ___ is tall plays basketball well.

  10. 10.

    The boy had big brown eyes and a pleasant ___.

  11. 11.

    Because of the rain yesterday, the children ___ inside the house.

  12. 12.

    Nancy knocked ___ before entering the house.

  13. 13.

    The children put on their boots ___ it snows.

  14. 14.

    I want to play with a toy ___ is fun.

  15. 15.

    ___ is Susan going to the doctor today?

  16. 16.

    Jeffrey wanted to go ___ the roller coaster.

  17. 17.

    When we go ___ the building, we must be quiet.

  18. 18.

    Dad ___ Bobby a letter several weeks ago.

  19. 19.

    After her broken leg had healed, Laura found it hard to walk ___.

  20. 20.

    Paul’s mother picked up the toys ___ books

Appendix 3

Prefixes/suffixes knowledge—grade 7

You will see a list of parts of words. Try to guess what they mean. Think of a word that has that part in it and write it on the line. Some of them are at the beginning of words and some are at the end. Some of these are hard but try your best. This is not a test. Your teacher will not see your answers.

Example

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lipka, O., Siegel, L.S. The development of reading comprehension skills in children learning English as a second language. Read Writ 25, 1873–1898 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9309-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9309-8

Keywords

Navigation