Skip to main content
Log in

Use orthography in L2 auditory word learning: Who benefits?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effect of capitalizing on orthography in auditory learning of English words was examined in 74 children who spoke Mandarin Chinese as their primary language. To use orthographic information for auditory word learning, children must recode printed words phonologically to assist the reconstruction of the speech single misheard or underspecified, an ability that may depend heavily on phonological awareness (PA). In this study, children with poorer PA of Chinese and those with better PA were taught novel English words in an auditory learning task under two exposure conditions: auditory words presented with their written forms and auditory words presented with undecodable symbols. Word learning performance was better in the written form condition than in the symbol condition, but the effect was smaller for children with poorer PA. The facilitative effect was associated with L2 PA for children with poorer PA but not for children with better PA. The results are discussed with regard to how poor PA may constrain auditory word learning in an L2 context.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akamatsu, N. (2003). The effects of first language orthographic features on second language reading in text. Language Learning, 53, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, S. A., & Williams, J. N. (2002). The effect of bimodal input in implicit and explicit memory: An investigation into the benefits of within-language subtitling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 509–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjaalid, I.-K., Hoien, T., & Lundberg, I. (1996). The contribution of orthographic and phonological processes to word reading in young Norwegian readers. Reading and Writing, 8(2), 189–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 44–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Denton, C., & Gray, L. C. (2004). Categorical perception of speech stimuli in children at risk for reading difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 152–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, D. (2003). Language specific speech perception and the onset of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 573–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A one-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1, 104–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., Holmes, V. M., Neath, J., & Kinoshita, S. (2003). How does orthographic knowledge influence performance on PA tasks? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 56(A), 445–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948–958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., & Chen, H. C. (2004). Early orthographic experience modifies both phonological awareness and on-line speech processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(1), 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Chen, H.-C., Lai, C. Y., Wong. O. C., & Hills, M. (2001). The development of phonological awareness effects of spoken language experience and orthography. Cognition, 81, 227–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chow, B. W.-Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Burgess, S. (2005). Phonological processing skills and early reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese Kindergarteners learning to read English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 81–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., Seveke, M.-J., & van Veen, M. (2000). Phonological sensitivity and the acquisition of new words in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 275–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, R. S., Yekovich, F. R., & Gray, J. W. (1988). The effect of modality on long-term recognition memory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 102–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of PA. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children’s production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1993). How English orthography influences phonological knowledge as children learn to read and spell. In R. J. Scholes (Ed.), Literacy and language analysis (pp. 21–43). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., & Wilce, L. (1979). The mnemonic value of orthography among beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 26–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., & Wilce, L. S. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers’ conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1986). The influence of spellings on speech: Are alveolar flaps/d/ or /t/? In D. Yaden & S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning literacy (pp. 101–114). Exeter: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C. (1996). Early literacy abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 453–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Jesen, M. N. (2005). Quality of phonological representations, verbal learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erdener, V. D., & Burnham, D. K. (2005). The role of audiovisual speech and orthographic information in nonnative speech production. Language Learning, 55, 191–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C., Hunt, C., Chambers, K., & Church, B. (2001). Abstraction and specificity in preschoolers’ representations of novel spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 665–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 97–117). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A., McDermott, K. B., Percer, J., & Roediger, H. L. (2001). Modality effects in false recall and false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2), 339–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell, M.G., & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1996). Phonological variation and inference in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 144–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. (2000). Issues in the assessment of reading disabilities in L2 children – beliefs and research evidence. Dyslexia, 6, 13–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Understanding individual differences in word recognition skills of ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 123–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A. (2002). The relationship between language and reading skills in bilingual Spanish-English speakers. Topics in Language Disorders, 22(5), 46–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. L. (2004). Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 338–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohn, W. E., & Ehri, L. C. (1983). Do alphabet letters help prereaders acquire phonemic segmentation skill? Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 752–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C. F. (2003). Phonological memory, PA, and foreign language word learning. Language Learning, 53, 429–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C. F. (2004). Learning to spelling with poor PA. English Teaching and Learning, 28(3), 31–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C. F., & Schuele, C. M. (2005). Learning nonnative names: The effect of poor native PA. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 343–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). PA and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 73–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1997). A longitudinal study of phonological awareness, visual skills, and Chinese reading acquisition among first-graders in Taiwan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 259–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 30–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K., Tan, C. H., Cheng, P. W., & Hau, K. T. (2005). Learning to read and spell English words by Chinese students. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 63–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revisited. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lund, R. (1991). A comparison of second language listening and reading comprehension. The Modern Language Journal, 75, 196–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, C., Scobbie, J. M., Hewlett, N., & Waters, D. (2003). The influence of phonemic awareness development on acoustic cue weighting strategies in children’s speech perception. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1184–1196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Pseudoname learning by German-speaking children with dyslexia: Evidence for a phonological learning deficit. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 116–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is PA? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1996). Models of speech perception and phonological processing in reading. Child Development, 67, 1836–1856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Castro, M. S. L., Scliar-Carbral, L., Kolinsky, R., & Content, A. (1987). The effects of literacy on the recognition of dichotic words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 451–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, V. (1997). The effect of modality on a grammaticality judgment task. Second Language Research, 13, 34–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, J. R., Balass, M., & Perfetti, C. A. (2005). Differences between written and spoken input in learning new words. Written Language & Literacy, 8, 25–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nittrouer, S. (1996). The relation between speech perception and phonemic awareness: Evidence from low-SES children and children with chronic OM. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1059–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. (1992). The nonword reading deficits in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read, C. A., Zhang, Y., Nie, H., & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic reading. Cognition, 24, 31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 321–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serniclaes, W., van Heghe, S., Mousty, P., Carre, R., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of PA and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37, 886–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow C. E., Burns S., Griffin P. (Eds). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Bourassa, D. (2000). Children’s written and oral spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Cassar, M. (1997). Can children and adults focus on sound as opposed to spelling in a phoneme counting task? Developmental Psychology, 33, 771–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trofimovich, P., Gatbonton, E., & Segalowitz, N. (2007). A dynamic look at L2 phonological learning: Seeking processing explanations for implicational phenomena. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29(3), 407–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bus, A. G. (1994). Meta-analytic confirmation of the nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 266–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandervelden, M., & Siegel, L. (2001). Phonological processing in written word learning: Assessment for children who use augmentative and alternative communication. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 17, 37–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ventura, P., Kolinsky, R., Brito-Mendes, C., & Morais, J. (2001). Mental representations of the syllable internal structure are influenced by orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(4), 393–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. T. (1990). Acquisition of reading in a second language. Reading Research Quarterly, 25, 90–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children’s spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Koda, K., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Alphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: A comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition, 87, 129–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A, & Liu, Y. (2005). Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97, 67–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W. (2001). Modality and attention to meaning and form in the input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 345–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psychological grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this paper was supported by grant from the National Science Council of Taiwan (NSC892411H133010; NSC902411H133010; NSC912411H133006; NSC922411H133002). The authors thank the people and the institutions that helped locate the children, the parents who gave their consent, and the children who participated in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chieh-Fang Hu.

Appendix

Appendix

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hu, CF. Use orthography in L2 auditory word learning: Who benefits?. Read Writ 21, 823–841 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9094-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9094-6

Keywords

Navigation