Skip to main content
Log in

Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The paper reported an exploratory study that tested (a) the relationship between phonological and morphological awareness in English (L1)–Arabic (L2) bilingual children in Canada (N = 43), and (b) the relevance of these skills to word and pseudoword reading accuracy, and to complex word reading fluency. The results showed a significant correlation between phonological awareness in English and in Arabic. However, morphological awareness in the two languages was not correlated. Phonological awareness predicted reading cross-linguistically, but only Arabic morphological awareness predicted word reading in English. Moreover, while both phonological and morphological awareness in English predicted independent unique variance in English word reading, only phonological awareness in Arabic predicted Arabic word reading. Complex-word reading fluency was predicted by morphological awareness within both languages. Similarly, in both languages, phonological awareness was the single factor predicting pseudoword decoding accuracy. The results are discussed in terms of cross-linguistic differences between English and Arabic in orthographic depth and in morphological structure and transparency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. A. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arab-Moghaddam, N., & Senechal, M. (2001). Orthographic and phonological processing skills in reading and spelling in Persian/English bilinguals. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 140–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Assink, E. H. H., Vooijs, C., & Knuijt, P. P.N. A. (2000). Prefixes as access units in visual word recognition: A comparison of Italian and Dutch data. Reading and Writing, 12, 149–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, S. K., Simmons, D. C., & Kameeuni, E. J. (1998). Vocabulary acquisition: Research bases. In D. C. Simmons & E. J. Kammeuni (Eds.), What reading research tells us about children with diverse learning needs: Bases and basics (pp. 183–217). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence.

  • Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonologic, and morphologic skill in reading disabled and normal children: Evidence from perception and production of spoken Hebrew. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 876–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., & Feldman, L. B. (1990). The contribution of morphological and semantic relatedness to repetition priming at short and long lags: Evidence from Hebrew. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 693–711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Morphological factors in visual word recognition in Hebrew. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 217–292). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., & Shimron, J. (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the contour principle. Cognition, 64, 39–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1985). The acquisition of Hebrew. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross- linguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 1 (pp. 25–371). Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Berman, R. A. (1993). Developmental perspectives on transitivity: A confluence of cues. In Y. Levy (Ed.), Other children, other languages: Issues in the theory of acquisition (pp. 189–241). Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A (1999). Children’s innovative verbs versus nouns: Structured elicitations and spontaneous coinages. In L. Menn & N. Bernstein-Ratner (Eds.), Methods in studying language production (pp. 69–93). Mahhwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bindman, M. (2004). Grammatical awareness across languages and the role of social context: Evidence from English and Hebrew. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy. (pp. 691–709). Great Britian: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brittain, M. (1970). Inflectional performance and early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 34–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Aidinis, A. (1999). Different morphemes, same spelling problems: Cross-linguistic developmental studies. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 112–133). Cambridge University Press.

  • Byrne, B. (1996). The learnability of the alphabetic principle: Children’s initial hypotheses about how print represents spoken language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 401–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Laudanna, A., & Romani, C. (1988). Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 28, 297–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth and eighth grades. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. New York: American Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casalis, S., Cole, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C., Wang, M., & Chen, S. (2005). Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Toronto, Canada.

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cisero, C., & Royer, J. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 313–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V., & Berman, R. A. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word formation. Language, 60, 542–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, H. S., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, H. S. & Wade-Wolley, L. (2005). How the relationship between morphological awareness and reading changes as language skills develop. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Toronto, Canada.

  • Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Clark-Chiarelli, N., & Wolf, A. (2004). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 323–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doctor, E. A. & Klein, D. (1992). Phonological processing in bilingual word recognition. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals. (pp. 237–252). Amsterdam: North Holland.

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y., & Oney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing, 11, 281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang Hu, C., & Catts, H. W. (1993). Phonological recoding as a universal process? Evidence from beginning readers of Chinese. Reading and Writing, 5, 325–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M., & Totereau, C. (2001). Learning the written morphology of plural in written French. In L. Tolcjinski (Ed.), Developmental aspects in learning to write. (pp. 151–173). Doredrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M., Hupet, M., & Largy, P. (1999). The acquisition of subject-verb agreement in written French: From novices to experts errors. Reading and Writing, 11, 153–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M., Thenevin, M. G., Jarousse, J. P., & Totereau, C. (1999). From learning to teaching French written morphology. In T. Nunes (Ed.), Learning to read: An integrated view from research and practice (pp. 43–64).

  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Forster, K., & Deutsch, A. (1997). What can we learn from the morphology of Hebrew? A masked-priming investigation of morphological representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 829–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F. & Geva, E. (2006). Cross-linguistic relationships in working memory, phonological processes, and oral language. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second language learners: A report of the national literacy panel on language minority children and youth (Chapter 7, pp. 175–184). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Geva, E., & Siegel, S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Wade-Woolley, L. (2004). Issues in the assessment of reading disability in second language children. In I. Smythe (Ed.), The international book of dyslexia (pp. 195–206). John Wiley.

  • Geva, E., & Wang. M. (2001). The development of basic reading skills in children: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 182–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Wade-Woolley, L., & Shany, M. (1993). The concurrent development of spelling and decoding in different orthographies. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 383–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Wade-Woolley, L., & Shany, (1997). Development of reading efficiency in first and second language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 119–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Yaghoubzadeh, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Individual differences in word recognition skills of ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 123–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gholamain, M., & Geva, E. (1999). The concurrent development of word recognition skills in English and Farsi. Language Learning, 49, 183–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. United Kingdom: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ku, Y. M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition 63, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1989). Productive knowledge of derivational rules in poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 94–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (2000). Rapid processing of base and derived forms of words and grades 4, 5 and 6 children’s spelling. Reading and Writing, 12, 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Ravid, D., & Rapaport, S. (1999). Developing morphological awareness and learning to write: A two-way street. In T. Nunes (Ed.), Learning to read: An integrated view from research and practice. (pp. 77–104). Amsterdam: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I., Liberman, A. M., Mattingly, I., & Shankweiler, D. (1980). Orthography and the beginning reader. In R. L. Venesky & Kavanagh J. F. (Eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 137–153). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Growth and predictive relations of vocabulary and inflectional morphology in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. (2000). Introduction to the special issue on morphology and the acquisition of alphabetic writing systems. Reading and Writing, 12, 143–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101, 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., & Shu, H. (2005). The role of morphological awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. (1981). A prosodic theory of non-concatenative morphology. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 373–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohany, D. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 6, 19–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohany, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing, 12, 191–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quitregard, D. (1994). Arabic key words. USA: The Oleander Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2002). A developmental perspective on root perception in Hebrew, Palestinian Arabic. In Y. Shimron (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Semitic, root-based morphology (pp. 293–319). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Farah, R. (1999). Learning about noun plurals in Palestinian Arabic. First Language, 19, 187–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Malenky, D. (2001). Awareness of linear and non-linear morphology in Hebrew: A developmental study. First Language 21, 25–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Schiff, R. (2004). Learning to represent vowels in written Hebrew: Different factors across development. First Language, 24, 185–208. .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosner, J., & Simon, D. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003a). Bilingual oral reading fluency and reading comprehension: The case of Arabic/Hebrew (L1)-English (L2) readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 717–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003b). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 115–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 495–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing, 18, 559–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2004). Representing written vowels in university students with dyslexia compared with normal Hebrew readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 39–64. .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy skills in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (1972). Misreading: A search for causes. In J. F. Kavanaugh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by eye and by ear. (pp. 293–317). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singson, M., Mohany, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12, 219–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. National Research Council.

  • Stanovich, K. E (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. (1994). Interactive-activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 271–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Cassar, M. (1996). Effects of morphology on children’s spelling of final consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 141–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2003). Introduction to this special issue: The role of morphology in learning to read. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 209–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wade-Woolley, L., & Siegel, L. (1997). The spelling performance of ESL and native speakers of English as a function of reading skills. Reading and Writing, 9, 387–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, T. G., Power, M. A., & White, S. (1989). Morphological analysis: Implications for teaching and understanding vocabulary growth. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 283–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test- revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 66–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to E. Saiegh-Haddad.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Saiegh-Haddad, E., Geva, E. Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Read Writ 21, 481–504 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x

Keywords

Navigation