Skip to main content
Log in

Attentional capture by spatiotemporally task-irrelevant faces: supportive evidence for Sato and Kawahara (2015)

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Visual attention is captured exogenously by stimuli that are congruent with the viewer’s current behavioral goals or intentions. However, Sato and Kawahara (Psychol Res 79:523–533, 2015) recently suggested that distractor faces capture attention in an entirely stimulus-driven manner without top-down control of attention, which then attenuates subsequent target identification, using a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm. We tested this suggestion, developing a hypothesis that the faces used in the previous study served as task-relevant temporal cues that predicted target timing. To evaluate this hypothesis, we eliminated the task relevance by widely varying distractor-target temporal lags (Experiment 1) and by counterbalancing the distractor-target temporal order (Experiment 2). In both experiments, the deterioration in performance resulting from attentional capture by the peripheral distractor face preceding the target remained robust; this effect was, however, eliminated when the face was inverted (Experiment 3). The present results provide clear evidence that upright faces capture attention exogenously even when they are spatiotemporally task irrelevant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ariga, A., & Watanabe, K. (2009a). Temporal dissociation between category-based and item-based processes in rejecting distractors. Psychological Research, 73, 54–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ariga, A., & Watanabe, K. (2009b). What is special about the index finger?: The index finger advantage in manipulating reflexive attentional shift. Japanese Psychological Research, 51, 258–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariga, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2008). Contingent attentional capture occurs by activated target congruence. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 680–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., Hooge, I. T. C., Jenkins, R., & DeHaan, E. H. F. (2005). Faces retain attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1048–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bindemann, M., Burton, A. M., Langton, S. R. H., Schweinberger, S. R., & Doherty, M. J. (2007). The control of attention to faces. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S. E., & Abrams, R. A. (2006). Abrupt onsets cannot be ignored. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 875–880.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 7426–7435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davoli, C. C., Suszko, J. W., & Abrams, R. A. (2007). New objects can capture attention without a unique luminance transient. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 338–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers automatic visuospatial orienting in adults. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychologics, 63, 1004–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2002). Made you blink!: Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Perception and Psychophysics, 64, 741–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone, A. (1992). Combining expectancies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 69–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, M. R. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 138–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (2000). You must see the point: Automatic processing of cues to the direction of social attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 747–757.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langton, S. R. H., Law, A. S., Burton, A. M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Attention capture by faces. Cognition, 107, 330–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Ro, T., & Russell, C. (2003). The role of perceptual load in processing distractor faces. Psychological Science, 14, 510–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2006). It’s under control: top-down search strategies can override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 132–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, W. S., & Mebane, M. W. (2006). Attentional capture triggers an attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 125–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oonk, H. M., & Abrams, R. A. (1998). New perceptual objects that capture attention produce inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 510–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention. Psychological Science, 7, 129–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ro, T., Russell, C., & Lavie, N. (2001). Changing faces: A detection advantage in the flicker paradigm. Psychological Science, 12, 94–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, T. A. (1960). Significant tests for multiple comparison of proportions, variances, and other statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 57, 318–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sato, S., & Kawahara, J. I. (2015). Attentional capture by completely task-irrelevant faces. Psychological Research, 79, 523–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13, 657–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyble, B., Folk, C., & Potter, M. C. (2013). Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 861–871.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 95–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP20609565 to Atsunori Ariga.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Atsunori Ariga.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ariga, A., Arihara, K. Attentional capture by spatiotemporally task-irrelevant faces: supportive evidence for Sato and Kawahara (2015). Psychological Research 82, 859–865 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0869-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0869-3

Navigation