Skip to main content
Log in

Measurement and reliability of nonverbal behavior

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This review synthesizes data on the reliability of nonverbal behaviors, classified according to kinesics, proxemics, haptics, vocalics, turn-taking behaviors, and global assessments, across a wide range of studies. The purpose of the review was (1) to determine which nonverbal behaviors within each classification can be measured reliably and (2) to determine which codes/classifications can be reliably measured. It was found that: (1) most nonverbal behaviors within each classification, except vocalics, can be measured reliably (.80+), (2) the codes themselves, when considered in aggregate form, can be measured reliably (.80+) when using the median as the best estimate. Several qualifications and recommendations for future research are suggested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abele, A. (1986). Function of gaze in social interaction: Communication and monitoring.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10, 83–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Interpersonal effects of experimenter attractiveness, attire, and gender.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 435–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). I show how you feel: Motor mimicry as a communicative act.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. A. (1985). Conversational involvement and loneliness.Communication Monographs, 52, 218–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanck, P. D., & Rosenthal, R. (1984). Mediation of interpersonal expectancy effects: Counselor's tone of voice.Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 418–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boice, R., & Monti, P. M. (1982). Specification of nonverbal behaviors for clinical assessment.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 7, 79–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breed, G. (1972). The effect of intimacy: Reciprocity or retreat?British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 135–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E. (1956).Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1985). The relationship of verbal and nonverbal codes. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voigt (Eds.),Progress in communication sciences 6 (pp. 263–298). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Aho, L. (1982). Three field experiments on the effects of violations of conversational distance.Communication Monographs, 49, 71–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Coker, D. A., & Coker, R. A. (1986). Communicative effects of gaze behavior.Human Communication Research, 12, 495–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Kelley, D. L., Keeley, M. P., & Coker, D. A. (1987, November).The nature of arousal and nonverbal indices. Paper presented at the Speech Communication Association Convention, Boston.

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Koper, R. J. (1984). Nonverbal and relational communication associated with reticence.Human Communication Research, 10, 601–626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Manusov, V., Mineo, P., & Hale, J. L. (1985). Effects of gaze on hiring, credibility, attraction and relational message interpretation.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 133–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1985). Production principles for turn-taking in social interaction: Socially anxious is socially secure persons.Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 193–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N., & Streibel, M. J. (1979). Computer analysis of talk-silence sequences: The FIASSCO system.Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 11, 384–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, A. J. (1975). Eye contact, physical proximity and laughter: A re-examination of the equilibrium model of social intimacy.Social Behavior and Personality, 3, 143–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coker, D. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The nature of conversational involvement and nonverbal encoding patterns.Human Communication Research, 13, 463–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M., & Weitz, S. (1981). Sex differences in body movements and positions. In C. Mayo & N. M. Henley (Eds.),Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 81–92). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ducan, S., Jr., & Fiske, D. W. (1977).Face-to-face interaction: Research, methods, and theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebel, R. L. (1951). Estimation of the reliability of ratings.Psychometrika, 16, 407–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1982). Methods for measuring facial action. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),Handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 45–90). Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978).The facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process of person perception: Visual interaction in relationship to competition, sex, and need for affiliation.Journal of Personality, 31, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V., Gray, D., & Schuette, D. (1965). Visual behavior in a dyad as affected by interview content and sex of respondent.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 201–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V. (1985). Multichannel transmission of nonverbal behavior and the perception of powerful men: The presidential debates of 1976. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.),Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 183–206). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldstein, S. (1972). Temporal patterns of dialogue: Basic research and reconsiderations. In A. W. Siegman & B. Pope (Eds.),Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 91–113). New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1978). The effects of behavioral and cultural expectation cues on the perception of social episodes.European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 203–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, W. V., Ekman, P., & Wallbott, H. (1979). Measuring hand movements.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 97–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fugita, S. S. (1974). Effects of anxiety and approval on visual interaction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 586–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallois, C., & Markel, N. N. (1975). Turn taking: Social personality and conversational style.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1134–1140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, G. N., Kiesler, C. A., & Collins, B. E. (1969). Visual behavior and face-to-face distance during interaction.Sociometry, 32, 43–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, S., & Rosenthal, R. (1986). Self-touching behavior in the job interview: Antecedents and consequence.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10 65–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1979).Marital interaction: Experimental investigations. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. O., O'Hair, H. D., Cody, M. J., & Yen, C. (1985). Planning and control of behavior during deception.Human Communication Research, 11 335–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrigan, J. A., Oxman, T. E., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Rapport expressed through nonverbal behavior.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, B. J., Everitt, B. S., & Frith, C. D. (1978). The role of gaze in dialogue.Acta Psychologica, 42 453–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewes, D. E. (1985). Systematic biases in coded social interaction data.Human Communication Research, 11 554–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., & Feldstein, S. (1970).Rhythms of dialogue. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorgenson, D. O. (1975). Field study of the relationship between status discrepancy and proxemic behavior.Journal of Social Psychology, 97 173–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jurich, A. P., & Jurich, J. A. (1974). Correlations among nonverbal expressions of anxiety.Psychological Reports, 34 199–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, D. J., Langmeyer, D., & Lundgren, D. C. (1973). Eye-contact, distance, and affiliation: The role of observer bias.Sociometry, 36 390–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. (1981). Gender gestures: Sex, sex-role, and nonverbal communication. In C. Mayo & N. M. Henley (Eds.),Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 129–150). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. (1985). Postural mirroring and intergroup relations.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, T. A. (1981). Nonverbal and paraverbal control in dyads and triads: Sex or power differences?Social Psychology Quarterly, 44 49–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leffler, A., Gillespie, D. L., & Conaty, J. C. (1982). The effects of status differentiation on nonverbal behavior.Social Psychology Quarterly, 45 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libby, W. L. Jr., (1970). Eye contact and direction of looking as stable individual differences.Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 4 303–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1968a). Inference of attitudes from the posture, orientation, and distance of a communicator.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32 296–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1968b). Relationship of attitude to seated posture, orientation, and distance.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10 26–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1969). Methods and designs: Some referents and measures of nonverbal behavior.Behavior Research Methods and Instruments, 1 203–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P., Jackson, R. J., & Kirshnit, C. (1984). Looking, laughing, and smiling in dyads as a function of intimacy motivation and reciprocity.Journal of Personality, 52 261–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAndrew, F. T., Gold, J. A., Lenney, E., & Ryckman, R. M. (1984). Explorations in immediacy: The nonverbal system and its relationship to affective and situational factors.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8 210–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monti, P. M., Kolko, D. J., Fingeret, A. L., & Zwick, W. R. (1984). Three levels of measurement of social skill and social anxiety.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8 187–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, R. P., & McGinley, H. (1972). Looking as a measure of attraction.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2 267–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarre, D. (1982). Posture sharing in dyadic interaction.American Journal of Dance Therapy, 5 28–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noller, P. (1980). Gaze in married couples.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5 115–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Hair, H. D., Cody, M. J., & McLaughlin, M. L. (1981). Prepared lies, spontaneous lies, Machiavellianism, and nonverbal communication.Human Communication Research, 7 325–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Leary, M. J., & Gallois, C. (1985). The last ten turns: Behavior and sequencing in friends' and strangers' conversational findings.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9 8–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, R., Devoe, S., & Wiener, M. (1978). Social patterns of turn-taking: Nonverbal regulators.Journal of Communication, 28 38–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1973). Stability of nonverbal immediacy behaviors.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9 97–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1982). Conducting judgment studies. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),Handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 287–361). Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., Blanck, P. D., & Vannicelli, M. (1984). Speaking to and about patients: Predicting therapists' tone of voice.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52 679–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabatelli, R. M., & Rubin, M. (1986). Nonverbal expressiveness and physical attractiveness as mediators of interpersonal perceptions.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10 120–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1978). Personality inference rules in personality attribution from voice quality: The loud voice of extroversion.European Journal of Social Psychology, 8 467–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigelman, C. K., Adams, R. M., Meeks, S. R., & Purcell, M. A. (1986). Children's nonverbal responses to a physically disabled person.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10 173–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sillars, A. L., Coletti, S. F., Parry, D., & Rogers, M. A. (1982). Coding verbal conflict tactics: Nonverbal and perceptual correlates of the “avoidance-distributive-integrative” distinction.Human Communication Research, 9 83–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steckler, N. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Sex differences in nonverbal and verbal communication with bosses, peers, and subordinates.Journal of Applied Psychology, 70 157–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strongman, K. T., & Champness, B. G. (1968). Dominance hierarchies and conflict in eye contact.Acta Psychologica, 28 376–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J. (1971).Statistical principles in experimental design (2nd ed.). New York: Mc-Graw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baesler, E.J., Burgoon, J.K. Measurement and reliability of nonverbal behavior. J Nonverbal Behav 11, 205–233 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987254

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987254

Keywords

Navigation