Skip to main content

Affective Word Processing in Native and Nonnative English Speakers: A Neuropragmatic Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 686 Accesses

Part of the book series: The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series ((BMBBS))

Abstract

This chapter constitutes the empirical heart of the book. Two electrophysiological (EEG) experiments were designed to investigate how the build-up of linguistic context might impact neurocognitive responses to affective adjectives in the first (L1) and second (L2) language of proficient, immersed Polish-English bilinguals. Experiment 2, published elsewhere (Jończyk et al., Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 16(3):527–540, 2016), is distilled to its essence. In experiment 1, participants viewed noun-adjective pairs (e.g. war-dead), and were asked to determine whether the pair was related in meaning. In experiment 2, the same adjectives were embedded in natural sentential context (e.g. Incurably ill Patrick did not realise that in a month he would already be dead); here, participants decided whether the sentences were meaningful. This “pragmatic twist” in the design enabled for the first time to uncover robust differences in cortical activation to affective adjectives in L1 and L2 that revealed themselves both in the early (N400 modulation) and late (LPC modulation) stages of processing. This effect was limited to adjectives embedded in affectively salient sentences, providing evidence that affective experience is boosted when individuals read natural language, which, in turn, may reveal differences in affect processing in the bilinguals’ respective languages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Note that in the present study it turned out to be impossible to form semantically unrelated noun-adjective dyads that would be at the same time affectively congruent, as was the case for the neutral prime condition.

  2. 2.

    Only applicable in the analysis of the bilingual group.

References

  • American Electroencephalographic Society. (1994). American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines in electroencephalography, evoked potentials, and polysomnography. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 11(1), 1–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M. (2009). The proactive brain: Memory for predictions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1235–1243. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0310.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 893–912. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32(1), 104–128. doi:10.1006/jesp.1996.0005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2010). Reading emotional words within sentences: The impact of arousal and valence on event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78(3), 299–307. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2012). P1 and beyond: Functional separation of multiple emotion effects in word recognition. Psychophysiology, 49(7), 959–969. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01381.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1993). Electrophysiological evidence for task effects on semantic priming in auditory word processing. Psychophysiology, 30(2), 161–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581–607). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3), 401–423. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Berntson, G. G., & Coles, M. G. H. (1993). If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the event-related brain potential? Psychological Science (Wiley-Blackwell), 4(2), 108–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(1), 115–125. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Citron, F. M. M. (2012). Neural correlates of written emotion word processing: A review of recent electrophysiological and hemodynamic neuroimaging studies. Brain and Language, 122(3), 211–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Citron, F. M. M., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2013). Effects of valence and arousal on written word recognition: Time course and ERP correlates. Neuroscience Letters, 533, 90–95. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, M., Recio, G., & Jacobs, A. M. (2011). The time course of emotion effects in first and second language processing: A cross cultural ERP study with German-Spanish bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 351. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00351.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2002). Affective priming of semantic categorisation responses. Cognition and Emotion, 16(5), 643–666. doi:10.1080/02699930143000419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorjee, D., Lally, N., Darrall-Rew, J., & Thierry, G. (2015). Dispositional mindfulness and semantic integration of emotional words: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuroscience Research, 97, 45–51. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2015.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, Z., & Adelman, J. S. (2008). Automatic vigilance for negative words in lexical decision and naming: Comment on Larsen, Mercer, and Balota (2006). Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 8(4), 441–444. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.441; discussion 445–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, Z., & Verges, M. (2008). Freeze or flee? Negative stimuli elicit selective responding. Cognition, 108(2), 557–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. doi:10.1080/0269993004200024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischler, I., & Bradley, M. (2006). Event-related potential studies of language and emotion: Words, phrases, and task effects. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 185–203. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56009-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hahne, A. (2001). What’s different in second-language processing? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 251–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., Dunning, J. P., & Foti, D. (2009). Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: Time-course of the late positive potential. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(3), 505–510. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45(3), 487–498. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, C., Pauli, P., & Herbert, B. M. (2011). Self-reference modulates the processing of emotional stimuli in the absence of explicit self-referential appraisal instructions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 653–661. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq082.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D., Houwer, J. D., & Eelen, P. (1994). The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. Cognition & Emotion, 8(6), 515–533. doi:10.1080/02699939408408957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herring, D. R., White, K. R., Jabeen, L. N., Hinojos, M., Terrazas, G., Reyes, S. M., … Crites, S. L. J. (2013). On the automatic activation of attitudes: A quarter century of evaluative priming research. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1062–1089. doi:10.1037/a0031309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinojosa, J. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2010). Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: Behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 748–757.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P. J. (1988). Automatic and attentional processing: an event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming. Brain and Language, 35(1), 66–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, D. J., Lynn, S. K., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2009). Neurophysiological correlates of comprehending emotional meaning in context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2245–2262. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jończyk, R., Boutonnet, B., Musiał, K., Hoemann, K., & Thierry, G. (2016). The bilingual brain turns a blind eye to negative statements in the second language. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 527–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Humphries, C., Lee, T. W., McKeown, M. J., Iragui, V., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 163–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138–148. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kissler, J., Assadollahi, R., & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: Insights from ERP studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 147–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and attention in visual word processing—An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 75–83. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kissler, J., & Koessler, S. (2011). Emotionally positive stimuli facilitate lexical decisions-an ERP study. Biological Psychology, 86(3), 254–264. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kousta, S.-T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112(3), 473–481. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., & Puls, B. (2014). Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 17(3), 673–680. doi:10.1017/S1366728913000606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 213. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., Wodniecka, Z., & Brysbaert, M. (2015). SUBTLEX-PL: Subtitle-based word frequency estimates for polish. Behavior Research Methods, 47(2), 471–483. doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0489-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J.-R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 574–588. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matlin, M. W., & Stang, D. J. (1978). The Pollyanna principle: Selectivity in language, memory, and thought. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: An electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 205–220. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, E. M., & Vázquez, C. (2011). Will the glass be half full or half empty? Brain potentials and emotional expectations. Biological Psychology, 88(1), 131–140. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 723–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naumann, E., Bartussek, D., Diedrich, O., & Laufer, M. E. (1992). Assessing cognitive and affective information processing functions of the brain by means of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. Journal of Psychophysiology, 6(4), 285–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106(3), 226–254. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Opitz, B., & Degner, J. (2012). Emotionality in a second language: It’s a matter of time. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 1961–1967. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2766–2775. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2012). Affective processing in bilingual speakers: Disembodied cognition? International Journal of Psychology, 47(6), 405–428. doi:10.1080/00207594.2012.743665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ponari, M., Rodríguez-Cuadrado, S., Vinson, D., Fox, N., Costa, A., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). Processing advantage for emotional words in bilingual speakers. Emotion, 15(5), 644–652. doi:10.1037/emo0000061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohr, L., & Abdel Rahman, R. (2015). Affective responses to emotional words are boosted in communicative situations. NeuroImage, 109, 273–282. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Time course and task dependence of emotion effects in word processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–43. doi:10.3758/CABN.9.1.28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, S., & Kissler, J. (2016). People matter: Perceived sender identity modulates cerebral processing of socio-emotional language feedback. NeuroImage, 134, 160–169. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, S., Wegrzyn, M., Steppacher, I., & Kissler, J. (2014). It’s all in your head—How anticipating evaluation affects the processing of emotional trait adjectives. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1292. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01292.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, S., Wegrzyn, M., Steppacher, I., & Kissler, J. (2015). Perceived communicative context and emotional content amplify visual word processing in the fusiform gyrus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(15), 601–6019. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3346-14.2015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2003). Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychological Science, 14(1), 7–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 95–104. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12530–12535. doi:10.1073/pnas.0609927104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2010). The brain is a prediction machine that cares about good and bad—any implications for neuropragmatics. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 22(1), 181–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.850521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinson, D., Ponari, M., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). How does emotional content affect lexical processing? Cognition & Emotion, 28(4), 737–746. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.851068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191–1207. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions. Cognition and Emotion, 11(4), 433–465. doi:10.1080/026999397379872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. J., Athanassiou, S., Dorjee, D., Roberts, M., & Thierry, G. (2012). Brain potentials dissociate emotional and conceptual cross-modal priming of environmental sounds. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 22(3), 577–583. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2012). How reading in a second language protects your heart. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(19), 6485–6489. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6119-11.2012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yap, M. J., & Seow, C. S. (2014). The influence of emotion on lexical processing: Insights from RT distributional analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 526–533. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0525-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39(2), 117–123. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In J. P. Forgas & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 31–58). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Jończyk, R. (2016). Affective Word Processing in Native and Nonnative English Speakers: A Neuropragmatic Perspective. In: Affect-Language Interactions in Native and Non-Native English Speakers. The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47635-3_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics