Skip to main content
Log in

An Explanation for the Difficulty of Leading Conceptual Change Using a Counterintuitive Demonstration: The Relationship Between Cognitive Conflict and Responses

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Bringing successful teaching approaches for stimulating conceptual change to normal classrooms has been a major challenge not only for teachers but also for researchers. In this study, we focused on the relationship between cognitive conflict and responses to anomalous data when students are confronted with a counterintuitive demonstration in the form of a discrepant event. The participants in this study were 96 secondary school students (9th grade) from S. Korea. We investigated students’ preconceptions of motion by administering a written test. After the exam, we presented a demonstration that may have conflicted with the ideas held by students. We then investigated the relationship between students’ cognitive conflict and responses to anomalous data by using a Cognitive Conflict Level Test (CCLT). Results showed that cognitive conflict initiated the first step in the process of conceptual change. Anxiety was an especially crucial component of cognitive conflict, affecting the relationship between cognitive conflict and students’ responses. In addition, superficial conceptual change was found to be the most common response.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alvermann, D. E., & Hague, S. A. (1989). Comprehension of counterintuitive science text: effects of prior knowledge and text structure. Journal of Education Research, 82, 197–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodrakova, W. V. (1988). The role of external and cognitive conflict in children’s conservation learning. Unpublished doctorial dissertation, City University of New York.

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Champagne, A. B., Gunstone, R. F., & Kloper, L. E. (1985). Instructional consequences of students’ knowledge about physical phenomena. In L. H. T. West & A. L. Pines (Eds.), Cognitive structure and conceptual change (pp. 61–90). Orlando: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C., Burtis, J., & Bereiter, C. (1997). Knowledge building as a mediator of conflict in conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 623–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cizek, G. J., & Burg, S. S. (2006). Addressing test anxiety in a high-stakes environment: Strategies for classrooms and schools. Callifornia: Corwin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interaction and the process of change in children’s moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 347–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dekkers, P. J. J. M., & Thijs, G. D. (1998). Making productive use of students’ initial conceptions in developing the concept of force. Science Education, 82, 31–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, A., Jungwirth, E., & Eliovitch, R. (1990). Applying the “cognitive conflict” strategy for conceptual change–some implications, difficulties, and problems. Science Education, 74, 555–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druyan, S. (1997). Effect of the kinesthetic conflict on promoting scientific reasoning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34, 1083–1099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elby, A., & Hammer, D. (2001). On the substance of a sophisticated epistemology. Science Education, 85, 554–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elizabeth, L., & Galloway, D. (1996). Conceptual links between cognitive acceleration through science education and motivational style: a critique of Adey and Shyer. International Journal of Science Education, 18, 35–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, E. D., Yekovich, C. W., & Yekovich, F. R. (1993). The cognitive psychology of school learning. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorsky, P., & Finegold, M. (1994). The role of anomaly and of cognitive dissonance in restructuring students’ concepts of force. Instructional Science, 22, 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, D. (2000). Student resources for learning introductory physics. American Journal of Physics, suppl. 68, 52–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hashweh, M. Z. (1986). Toward an explanation of conceptual change. European Journal of Science Education, 8, 229–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haws, L., & Kiser, T. (1995). Exploring the Brachistochrone problem. American Mathematical Monthly, 102, 308–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hennessey, M. G. (1999, March). Probing the dimensions of metacognition: Implications for conceptual change teaching-learning. Paper presented at the 1999 NARST Annual Meeting, Boston, MA.

  • Hewson, P. W. (1981). A conceptual approach to learning science. European Journal of Science Education, 3, 383–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewson, P. W., & Hewson, M. G. A. (1984). The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change and the design of science instruction. Instructional Science, 13, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewson, P. W., & Thorley, N. R. (1989). The conditions of conceptual change in the classroom. International Journal of Science Education, 18, 35–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1979). Conflict in the classroom: controversy and Learning. Review of Educational Research, 49, 51–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S., Scharmann, L. C., & Noh, T. (2004). Reexaming the role of cognitive conflict in science concept learning. Research in Science Education, 34, 71–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S., Scharmann, L. C., Noh, T., & Koh, H. (2005). The influence of students’ cognitive and motivational variables in respect of cognitive conflict and conceptual change. International Journal of Science Education, 27, 1037–1058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, H., Scharmann, L. C., Kang, S., & Noh, T. (2010). Cognitive conflict and situational interest as factors influencing conceptual change. International Journal of Environmental & Science Education, 5, 383–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, J. (1989). A cognitive model of conceptual change in science learning. Physics Teaching (written in Korean), 7, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, J. (1997, May). The necessity of cognitive conflict strategy in science teaching. Paper presented at the International Conference on Science Education: Globalization of Science Education, Seoul, S. Korea.

  • Langfield-Smith, K. (1994). Cognitive map. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Human behaviour (pp. 647–653). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, G., & Kwon, J. (1999). Students’ responses confronted with discrepant situation patterns about inertia concept. Journal of Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 19(4), 516–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, G., Kwon, J., Park, S., Kim, J., Kwon, H., & Park, H. (2003). Development of an instrument for measuring cognitive conflict in secondary-level science classes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(6), 585–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, G., Shin, J., Park, J., Song, S., Kim, Y., & Bao, L. (2005). An integrated theoretical structure of mental models: Toward understanding how students form their ideas about science. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 25(6), 698–709.

    Google Scholar 

  • Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: a critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, J. (2007). Responses to anomalous data obtained from repeatable experiments in the laboratory. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44, 506–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2000). Role of anomalous data and epistemological beliefs in middle school students’ theory change about two controversial topics. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 15, 329–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2001). Responses to anomalous data on controversial topics and theory change. Learning and Instruction, 11, 453–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthew, M. R. (1999). Social constructivism and mathematics education: some comments. Philosophy of Education, 330–341.

  • McDermott, L. C., Shaffer, P. S., & Somers, M. D. (1994). Research as a guide for teaching introductory mechanics: an illustration in the context of the Atwood’s machine. American Journal of Physics, 62, 46–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mildenhall, P. T., & Williams, F. S. (2001). Instability in students’ use of intuitive and Newtonian models to predict motion: the critical effect of the parameters involved. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 643–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, T. (1971). Piaget: Cognitive conflict and the motivation of thought. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology (pp. 311–355). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misiti, F. L., & Shrigley, R. L. (1994). The role of cognitive dissonance on the science attitudes of middle school students. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 404109).

  • Mortimer, E. F., & Machado, A. H. (2000). Anomalies and conflicts in classroom discourse. Science Education, 84, 429–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, F. B. (1983). Equilibration as cognitive conflict. Developmental Review, 3, 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, F. B., Ames, G., & Botvin, G. (1977). The acquisition of conservation through cognitive dissonance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 519–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1995). Cognitive conflict as a teaching strategy in solving chemistry problems: a dialectic-constructivist perspective. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 959–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (2001). Response to contradiction: conflict resolution strategies used by students in solving problems of chemical equilibrium. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 10, 205–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (2006). Facilitating chemistry teachers’ understanding of alternative interpretations of conceptual change. Interchange, 37, 129–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structure: The central problem of intellectual development. Chicago: U of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenquist, M. L., & McDermott, L. C. (1987). A conceptual approach to teaching kinematics. American Journal of Physics, 55, 407–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E. (1979). On becoming a thinker: a psychoeducational model. Educational psychologist, 14, 70–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (1961). The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2, 156–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinner, A. (1994). The story of force: from Aristotle to Einstein. Physics Education, 29, 77–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S. (1972). Inducing cognitive development and learning: a review of short-term training experiments. Cognition, 1, 329–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorley, N. R., & Treagust, D. F. (1987). Conflict within dyadic interactions as a stimulant for conceptual change in physics. International Journal of Science Education, 9, 203–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treagust, D. F., & Duit, R. (2008). Conceptial change: a discussion of theoretical, methodological and practical challenges for science education. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 3, 297–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trowbridge, D. E., & McDermott, L. C. (1981). Investigation of student understanding of the concept of acceleration in one dimension. American Journal of Physics, 49, 242–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trumper, R. (1997). Applying conceptual conflict strategies in the learning of the energy concept. Research in Science and Technological Education, 15, 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, C. (2000). Enhancing science instruction: the use of “conflict maps”. International Journal of Science Education, 22, 285–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyson, L. M., Venville, G. J., Harrison, A. G., & Treagust, D. F. (1997). A multidimensional framework for interpreting conceptual change events in the classroom. Science Education, 81, 387–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venville, G. J., & Treagust, D. F. (1998). Exploring conceptual change in genetics using a multidimensional interpretive framework. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 1031–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Ioannides, C. (1998). From conceptual development to science education: a psychological point of view. International Journal of Science Education, 20, 1213–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wadsworth, B. J. (1996). Piaget’s theory of cognitive and affective development. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, L. H. T., & Pines, A. L. (1985). Cognitive structure and conceptual change. Orlando: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R., & Gunstone, R. (1989). Metalearning and conceptual change. International Journal of Science Education, 11, 577–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. R. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurological Psychology, 18, 459–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Blom, D. E. (1983). Toward an empirical test of the role of cognitive conflict in learning. Developmental Reviews, 3, 18–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zohar, A., & Aharon-Kravetsky, S. (2005). Exploring the effects of cognitive conflict and direct teaching for students of different academic levels. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42, 829–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gyoungho Lee.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lee, G., Byun, T. An Explanation for the Difficulty of Leading Conceptual Change Using a Counterintuitive Demonstration: The Relationship Between Cognitive Conflict and Responses. Res Sci Educ 42, 943–965 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-011-9234-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-011-9234-5

Keywords

Navigation