Skip to main content
Log in

Individual differences in mental rotation: what does gesture tell us?

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gestures are common when people convey spatial information, for example, when they give directions or describe motion in space. Here, we examine the gestures speakers produce when they explain how they solved mental rotation problems (Shepard and Meltzer in Science 171:701–703, 1971). We asked whether speakers gesture differently while describing their problems as a function of their spatial abilities. We found that low-spatial individuals (as assessed by a standard paper-and-pencil measure) gestured more to explain their solutions than high-spatial individuals. While this finding may seem surprising, finer-grained analyses showed that low-spatial participants used gestures more often than high-spatial participants to convey “static only” information but less often than high-spatial participants to convey dynamic information. Furthermore, the groups differed in the types of gestures used to convey static information: high-spatial individuals were more likely than low-spatial individuals to use gestures that captured the internal structure of the block forms. Our gesture findings thus suggest that encoding block structure may be as important as rotating the blocks in mental spatial transformation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Notes

  1. Two of these five individuals were in the low-spatial group based on their MRT-A scores.

  2. Although some researchers using MRT-A identify individuals as high ability if they score >50 % and low ability if they score ≤50 % (e.g., Geiser et al. 2006), we used the median split to divide participants into low- and high-scoring groups because of the distribution of scores in this sample (range: 0–87.5).

References

  • Alibali MW (2005) Gesture in spatial cognition: expressing, communicating and thinking about spatial information. Spat Cogn Comput 5:307–331

    Google Scholar 

  • Alibali MW, Heath DC, Myers HJ (2001) Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: some gestures are meant to be seen. J Mem Lang 44:169–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali MW, Spencer RC, Knox L, Kita S (2011) Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychol Sci 22:1138–1144

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amorim M-L, Isableu B, Jarraya M (2006) Embodied spatial transformations: “Body analogy” for the mental rotation of objects. J Exp Psychol Gen 135:327–347

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock SL, Goldin-Meadow S (2010) Gesture changes thought by grounding it in action. Psychol Sci 21:1605–1611

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock SL, Lyons IM, Mattarella-Micke A, Nusbaum HC, Small SL (2008) Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 105:13269–13273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Casey MB, Nuttall R, Pezaris E, Benbow CP (1995) The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Dev Psychol 31:697–705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chu M, Kita S (2008) Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. J Exp Psychol Gen 137:706–723

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chu M, Kita S (2011) The nature of gestures’ beneficial role in spatial problem solving. J Exp Psychol Gen 140:102–116

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S (1986) The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition 23:43–71

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Collins DW, Kimura D (1997) A large sex difference on a two- dimensional mental rotation task. Behav Neurosci 111:845–849

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper LA (1976) Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation. Percept Psychophys 19:296–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper LA, Shepard RN (1973) Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In: Chase WG (ed) Visual information processing. Academic Press, New York, pp 75–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich SB, Levine SC, Goldin-Meadow S (2006) The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Dev Psychol 42:1259–1268

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey K, Casey S (2001) Gesture, thought and spatial language. Gesture 1:35–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feyereisen P, Havard I (1999) Mental imagery and production of hand gestures while speaking in younger and older adults. J Nonverbal Behav 23:153–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick A, Daum MM, Walser S, Mast FW (2009) Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. J Cogn Dev 10:18–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garber P, Goldin-Meadow S (2002) Gesture offers insight into problem solving in adults and children. Cogn Sci 26:817–831

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geiser C, Lehmann W, Eid M (2006) Separating ‘Rotaters’ from ‘Non-Rotaters’ in the Mental Rotations Test: a multigroup latent class analysis. Multivar Behav Res 41:261–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S (2003) Hearing gesture: how our hands help us think. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S, Beilock SL (2010) Action’s influence on thought: the case of gesture. Perspect Psychol Sci 5:664–674

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S, Nusbaum H, Kelly SD, Wagner S (2001) Explaining math: gesturing lightens the load. Psychol Sci 12:516–522

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SL, Zinchenko E, Yip TK-Y, Hemani N, Factor L (in press) Doing gesture promotes learning a mental transformation task better than seeing gesture. Dev Sci

  • Hegarty M, Mayer S, Kriz S, Keehner M (2005) The role of gestures in mental animation. Spat Cogn Comput 5:333–335

    Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter AB, Alibali MW (2007) Raise your hand if you’re spatial: relations between verbal and spatial skills and gesture production. Gesture 7:73–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter AB, Alibali MW, Bartholomew AE (2011) Gesture during mental rotation. In: Carlson L, Hoelscher C, Shipley TF (eds) Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of cognitive science society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX, pp 1448–1453

  • Hyun J-S, Luck SJ (2007) Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation. Psychon Bull Rev 14:154–158

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jansen-Osmann P, Heil M (2007) Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain Cogn 64:217–227

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just MA, Carpenter PA, Maguire M, Diwadkar V, McMains S (2001) Mental rotation of objects retrieved from memory: a functional MRI study of spatial processing. J Exp Psychol Gen 130:493–504

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Margolis JA, Barrett AM, Goldknopf EJ, Daly PF (1990) Age differences in imagery abilities. Child Dev 61:995–1010

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lavergne J, Kimura D (1987) Hand movement asymmetry during speech: no effect of speaking topic. Neuropsychologia 25:689–693

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perry M, Elder AD (1997) Knowledge in transition: adults’ developing understanding of a principle of physical causality. Cogn Dev 12:131–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry M, Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S (1988) Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Cogn Dev 3:359–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters M, Laeng B, Latham K, Jackson M, Zaiyouna R, Richardson C (1995) A Redrawn Vandeberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test: different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain Cogn 28:39–58

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peters M, Lehmann W, Takaira S, Takeuchi Y, Jordan K (2006) Mental rotation test performance in four cross-cultural samples (N = 3367): overall sex differences and the role of Academic Program in performance. Cortex 42:1005–1014

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ping R, Goldin-Meadow S (2010) Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects. Cogn Sci 34:602–619

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rauscher FB, Krauss RM, Chen Y (1996) Gesture, speech, and lexical access: the role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychol Sci 7:226–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter A, Uttal D, Alman AS, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC (2012) Learning what children know about space from looking at their hands: the added value of gesture in spatial communication. J Exp Child Psychol 111:587–606

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz DL, Black JB (1996) Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: induction and fallback. Cogn Sci 20:457–497

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz DL, Black T (1999) Inferences through imagined actions: knowing by simulated doing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 25:116–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz DL, Holton DL (2000) Tool use and the effect of action on the imagination. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 26:1655–1665

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sekiyama K (1982) Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Percept Psychophys 32:89–95

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard R, Cooper L (1982) Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg SG, Kuse AR (1978) Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept Mot Skills 47:599–604

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vanetti EJ, Allen GL (1988) Communicating environmental knowledge: the impact of verbal and spatial abilities on the production and comprehension of route directions. Environ Behav 20:667–682

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wexler M, Kosslyn SM, Berthoz A (1998) Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition 68:77–94

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 9:625–636

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlschlager A, Wohlschlager A (1998) Mental and manual rotation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2:397–412

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright R, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Newcombe NS, Kosslyn SM (2008) Training generalized spatial skills. Psychon Bull Rev 15:763–771

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu L, Franconeri SL (in press) The head of the table: the location of the spotlight of attention may determine the ‘front’ of ambiguous objects. J Neurosci

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by a grant to the Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center, funded by the National Science Foundation (grant numbers SBE-0541957 and SBE-1041707), and by NICHD R01 HD47450 and NSF BCS-0925595 (to SGM), and by a National Science Foundation Fostering Interdisciplinary Research on Education grant (DRL- 1138619). We would like to thank Shannon Fitzhugh, Dominique Dumay for their assistance in collecting data, Michelle Chery for reliability coding, and the RISC group at Temple University for their helpful comments in this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tilbe Göksun.

Additional information

This article is part of the special issue on “Spatial Learning and Reasoning Processes”, guest-edited by Thomas F. Shipley, Dedre Gentner, and Nora S. Newcombe. Handling Editor of this manuscript: Dedre Gentner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Göksun, T., Goldin-Meadow, S., Newcombe, N. et al. Individual differences in mental rotation: what does gesture tell us?. Cogn Process 14, 153–162 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0549-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0549-1

Keywords

Navigation