Skip to main content
Log in

A short history of ideo-motor action

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ideo-motor theory, which is currently receiving heightened interest in cognitive psychology, looks back on a long history. Essentially two historical roots can be presented. A British one, initiated by Laycock (1845) and Carpenter (1852), which was developed in order to explain ideo-motor phenomena by means of cerebral reflex actions. A second and older root is the German one by Herbart (1816, 1825), Lotze (1852), and Harless (1861), which considered the ideo-motor principle a fundamental mechanism of all intentional human behaviour. Both roots converged in James’ (1890) Principles of Psychology before they fell into oblivion due to the dominance of behaviorism in the first half of the 20th century. The few empirical ideo-motor studies of the early 20th century are briefly described. Finally, similarities and differences in the history of the ideo-motor theory are delineated and a perspective is given covering research questions that could be examined in the future.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. According to Wozniak (1999) other members of this research group were: Benjamin Collins Brodie (1783–1862), Robert Dunn (1799–1877), Henry Holland (1788–1873), Thomas Laycock (1812–1876) John Daniel Morell (1816–1891), and Daniel Noble (1810–1885).

  2. In the history of psychology the conception of physiological psychology is usually ascribed to Wilhelm Wundt (1873/1874). This is not entirely correct when considering the work of the research-group around Carpenter.

  3. Nevertheless, Carpenter denied that Laycock had already described the triggering of cerebral reflexes by ideas and presented these findings of Laycock as his own. This resulted in an ongoing dispute concerning authorship between the two (cf. Carpenter, 1871; Laycock, 1860, 1876).

  4. But one could suppose that Lotze was confronted with the works of the anatomist Albrecht von Haller (1708–1777) during his medical studies. Professor von Haller held a position in the Department of Anatomy, Surgery, and Botany at the University of Göttingen from 1736 till 1753 (cf. Beug, 1977). As mentioned earlier, Laycock (1845) had also made reference to von Haller as a scholar who supported the substrate theory. Maybe von Haller is the connecting link between Laycock and Lotze.

References

  • Allers, R., & Scheminzky, F. (1926). Über Aktionsströme der Muskeln bei motorischen Vorstellungen und verwandten Vorgängen. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 212, 169–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beug, H.-J. (1977). Albrecht von Haller an der Georg-August-Universität Göttingen. In Albrecht von Haller. Zum 200. Todestag. Göttingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.

  • Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, W. B. (1852). On the influence of suggestion in modifying and directing muscular movement, independently of volition. Proceedings of the Royal Institution, 147–154.

  • Carpenter, W. B. (1871). Spiritualism and its recent converts. Quarterly Review, 131, 301–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. London: Herny S. King.

  • Carus, C. G. (1988). Über Lebensmagnetismus und über die magischen Wirkungen überhaupt. Andechs, Germany: Dingfelder.

  • Chevreul, M.-E. (1833). Lettre à M. Ampère sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires. Revue des Deux Mondes, 2, 249–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1990). Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 40–47.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 27, 229–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faraday, M. (1853). Experimental investigation of table-moving. Athenaeum, 1340, 801–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harless, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. In I. H. Fichte, H. Ulrici, & I. U. Wirth (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik: Vol. 38 (pp. 50–73). Halle, Germany: Pfeffer.

  • Herbart, J. F. (1816). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer.

  • Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. Zweiter, analytischer Teil. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer.

  • Herbart, J. F. (1834/1965). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Nachdruck der Ausgabe Königsberg 1834. Amsterdam: Bonset.

  • Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

  • Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000a). Antizipative Verhaltenskontrolle. In B. Strauss & J. Möller (Eds.), Psychologie und Zukunft (pp. 1–29). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

  • Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000b). Lernmechanismen zum Erwerb verhaltenssteuernden Wissens. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J., & Stock, A. (2000). Intention als psychischer Prozess—eine Suche nach Spuren in der allgemeinpsychologischen Forschung. In K. Friedhart (Ed.), Psychologie 2000 (pp. 1–24). Berlin, Germany: J. A. Barth.

  • Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1998). Perceiving one’s own action—and what it leads to. In J. S. Jordan (Ed.), Systems theory and a priori aspects of perception (pp. 143–179). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  • Hommel, B. (2003). Acquisition and control of voluntary action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, R. (1999). The mischief making of ideomotor action. Scientific Review of Alternative Medicine, 3, 30–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vols. I, II. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Külpe, O. (1889). Die Lehre vom Willen in der neueren Psychologie. In W. Wundt (Ed.), Philosophische Studien, Vol. 5. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.

  • Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laycock, T. (1840). A treatise on the nervous diseases of women; comprising an inquiry into the nature, causes, and treatment of spinal and hysterical disorders. London: Longmans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laycock, T. (1845). On the reflex functions of the brain. In Reprinted from N. XXXVII of The British and Foreign Medical Review (pp. 1–16). Bartholomew Close, UK: Adlard.

  • Laycock, T. (1860). Mind and brain: or the correlations of consciousness and organisation; with their applications to philosophy, zoology, physiology, mental pathology, and the practice of medicine, Vols. I, II. Edinburgh/London: Sutherland and Knox/Simpkin, Marshall, and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laycock, T. (1876). Reflex, automatic, and unconscious cerebration: A history and a criticism. The Journal of Mental Science, 21, 477–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotze, R. H. (1841). Metaphysik. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann.

  • Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele (pp. 287–325). Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.

  • Moede, W. (1920). Experimentelle Massenpsychologie. Beiträge zur Experimentalpsychologie der Gruppe. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.

  • Nath, M. (1892). Die Psychologie Hermann Lotzes in ihrem Verhältnis zu Herbart. Inaugural Dissertation. Halle a.S., Germany.

  • Pesters, R. (1997). Hermann Lotze. Wege seines Denkens und Forschens. Würzburg, Germany: Königshausen & Neumann.

  • Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo-motor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–76). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1990a). Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 326–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1990b). The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 262–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1991a). Associations of multiple outcomes with instrumental response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Processes, 17, 465–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1991b). Associative relations in instrumental learning: The Eighteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43b, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, H. (1954). Über ideomotorische Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 157, 201–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, H. (1957). Zum Problem der ideomotorischen Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 161, 161–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. (1898). Dreidimensionale Analyse von Ausdrucksbewegungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 16, 275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, H. H. (1997). Nonconscious movements. From mystical messages to facilitated communication. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Stock, A. (2002). Intentionalität und Ideo-Motorik. Eine handlungstheoretisch-psychologische Synthese. Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, Habilitationsschrift.

  • Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R-O learning blocks S-R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Ideo-motor action. Psychological Review, 20, 91–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tischner, R. (1929). Zur Geschichte des ideomotorischen Prinzips. Zeitschrift für Parapsychologie, 56, 75–85; 155–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wentscher, M. (1925). Fechner und Lotze. Munich, Germany: Ernst Reinhardt.

  • Wozniak, R. H. (1999). Classics in psychology. 1855–1914: Historical essays. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.

  • Wundt, W. (1873/1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelmann.

  • Young, R. M. (1990). The mind-body problem. In: R. C. Olby et al. (Eds.), Companion to the history of modern science. London: Routledge.

  • Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 595–613.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Peter A. French and Wolfgang Prinz for their important and constructive comments on an earlier version of this paper. Additionally, Viola Rost and Christian Stöcker should be mentioned for their help in checking and improving the English. Last but not least, many thanks to Christian Stöcker especially for translating the old German quotations from Herbart, Lotze, and Harless.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Armin Stock.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stock, A., Stock, C. A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research 68, 176–188 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0154-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0154-5

Keywords

Navigation