Skip to main content

What Counts as Science in Everyday and Family Interactions?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Explorations of Educational Purpose ((EXEP,volume 23))

Abstract

What counts as science? As evidenced by the focus of this entire volume, this is a complex and controversial question and the answer necessarily depends on the contexts and the stakeholders involved (Stevens, 2000). Other chapters in this volume take the valuable step of examining in some depth how various stakeholders seem to count certain talk and action as science (see chapter by Stevens). Our goal in this chapter is somewhat distinct from the other chapters in this section, because our starting place is different. We ask how children first encounter and begin to think about science without even knowing about science as a domain (see also National Research Council [NRC], 2007, 2009). And we ask how, while engaging in a variety of everyday activities, parents may contribute to children’s early thinking about science, sometimes without even meaning to do so (see also Ash, 2003; Crowley & Galco, 2001; Ellenbogen, 2002).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aikenhead, G. S. (2001). Students’ ease in crossing cultural borders into school science. Science Education, 85(2), 180–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ash, D. (2003). Dialogic inquiry in life science conversations of family groups in a museum. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40, 138–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briggs, R. (1978). The snowman. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M., & Jipson, J. L. (2001). Explanatory conversations and young children’s developing scientific literacy. In K. Crowley, C. D. Schuam, & T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for science: Implications from everyday, classroom, and professional settings (pp. 21–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M., Jipson, J., & Soennichsen, M. (2002). Maps, globes, and videos: Parent-child conversations about representational objects. In S. Paris (Ed.), Perspectives on object-centered learning in museums (pp. 261–283). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. A., & Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7, 213–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. A., & Valle, A. (2008). Co-constructing conceptual domains through family conversations and activities. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Advances in Research and Theory, Vol. 49, pp. 147–165). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cimpian, A., & Markman, E. M. (2008). Preschool children’s use of cues to generic meaning. Cognition, 107, 19–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., Callanan, M., Jipson, J., Galco, J., Topping, K., & Shrager, J. (2001). Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent-child activity. Science Education, 85, 712–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., & Galco, J. (2001). Everyday activity and the development of scientific thinking. In K. Crowley, C. Schunn, & T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for science: Implications for everyday, classroom, and professional settings (pp. 393–413). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., & Jacobs, M. (2002). Building islands of expertise in everyday family activity. In G. Leinhardt, K. Crowley, & K. Knutson (Eds.), Learning conversations in museums (pp. 333–356). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (1993). Ontologies in pieces: Response to Chi and Slotta. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 261–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (2000). Changing minds: Computers, learning, and literacy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R. (1985). Beyond appearances: The conservation of matter under physical and chemical transformations. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science (pp. 145–169). Philadelphia: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. (1995). How scientists really reason: Scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 365–395). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, K. M. (2002). Museums in family life: An ethnographic case study. In G. Leinhardt, K. Crowley, & K. Knutson (Eds.), Learning conversations in museums (pp. 81–101). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. H., & Dierking, L. D. (1992). The museum experience. Washington, DC: Whalesback Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K. S., Hartman, E., & Pappas, A. (1998). Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(1), Serial No. 253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S., & Raman, L. (2003). Preschool children use linguistic form class and pragmatic cues to interpret generics. Child Development, 74(1), 308–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, M. E., & Schauble, L. (2000). Parents’ assistance of their children’s scientific reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 17, 343–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutiérrez, K., Baquedano-Lopez, P., & Tejeda, C. (1999). Rethinking diversity: Hybridity and hybrid language practices in the third space. Mind, Culture & Activity: An International Journal, 6(4), 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educational Researcher, 32, 19–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77, 505–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2002). Young children’s naïve thinking about the biological world. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. (2003). Mother-child conversation and children’s understanding of biological and non-biological changes in size. Child Development, 74, 629–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jipson, J. L., & Gelman, S. A. (2007). Robots and rodents: Children’s inferences about living and nonliving kinds. Child Development, 78, 1675–1688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jipson, J.L., Clark, L., Etcheverry, R., Jones, H., Lebs, J., Reid G., Slavik, L., & Ziemba, R. (2009). Parent-child considerations of science and health. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development. Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. E., Scott, P., & Mervis, C. B. (2004). What are theories for? Concept use throughout the continuum of dinosaur expertise. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 171–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lautrey, J., & Mazens, K. (2004). Is children’s naïve knowledge consistent? A comparison of the concepts of sound and heat. Learning and Instruction, 14, 399–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leibham, M., Alexander, J., Johnson, K., Neitzel, C., & Reis-Henrie, F. (2005). Parenting behaviors associated with the maintenance of preschoolers’ interests: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 397–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luce M. R., & Callanan, M. A. (2010). “Heateddiscussions: Family conversations about heat and temperature. Manuscript under review [unpublished paper].

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (2007). In R. A. Duschl, H. A. Schweingruber, & A. W. Shouse (Eds.), Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8 (Committee on Science Learning, K-8). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (2009). In P. Bell, B. Lewenstein, A. W. Shouse, & M. A. Feder (Eds.), Learning science in informal environments: People, places, and pursuits (Committee on Learning Science in Informal Environments). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Gonzales, P., & Jacoby, S. (1996). “When I come down I’m in a domain state”: Talk, gesture, and graphic representation in the interpretive activity of physicists. In E. Ochs, E. Schegloff, & S. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 328–369). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Taylor, C., Rudolph, D., & Smith, R. (1992). Storytelling as a theory-building activity. Discourse Processes, 15, 37–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, R. J., & Cosgrove, M. M. (1983). Children’s conceptions of the changes of state of water. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20, 825–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmquist, S., & Crowley, K. (2007). From teachers to testers: How parents talk to novice and expert children in a natural history museum. Science Education, 91, 783–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigney, J., & Callanan, M. (2011). Patterns in parent-child conversations about animals at a marine science center. Cognitive Development, 26, 155–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Calabrese Barton, A. (2004). Rethinking scientific literacy. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Slotta, J. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology training. Cognition and Instruction, 24, 261–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, R. (2000). Who counts what as math? Emergent and assigned mathematics problems in a project-based classroom. In J. Boaler (Ed.), Multiple perspectives on mathematics teaching and learning. Westport, CT: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarlowski, A. (2006). If it’s an animal it has axons: Experience and culture in preschool children’s reasoning about animates. Cognitive Development, 21, 249–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum, H., & Callanan, M. (2008). Parents’ science talk to their children in Mexican-descent families residing in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triona, L. M., & Callanan, M. A. (2008). Science play: Comparing children alone, with peers, and with adults. In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (p. 1378). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valle, A. (2005). “How do you know?Communicating ideas about science and scientific reasoning in parent-child conversations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, B., Ballenger, C., Ogonowski, M., Rosebery, A., & Hudicourt-Barnes, J. (2001). Rethinking diversity in learning science: The logic of everyday sense-making. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 529–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher psychological processes. A clarification and application of Vygotsky’s theory. Human Development, 22, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M. (1995). Use of history of science to understand and remedy students’ misconceptions about heat and temperature. In D. N. Perkins, J. L. Schwartz, M. M. West, & M. S. Wiske (Eds.), Software goes to school: Teaching for understanding with new technologies (pp. 23–38). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M., & Amin, T. (2001). Is heat hot? Inducing conceptual change by integrating everyday and scientific perspectives on thermal phenomena. Learning and Instruction, 11, 331–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M., & Carey, S. (1983). When heat and temperature were one. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 267–297). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Center for Informal Learning and Schools (ESI-0119787), as well as research grants from NSF (ESI-0229872), NICHHO (26228), NIH (T32HD046423) and an AERA-IES Postdoctoral Research Fellowship to L. Triona. We are grateful to children’s Discovery Museum of San Jose and Seymoul Marine Discovery Center for their Collaboration.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maureen Callanan .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Callanan, M., Luce, M., Triona, L., Rigney, J., Siegel, D., Jipson, J. (2013). What Counts as Science in Everyday and Family Interactions?. In: Bevan, B., Bell, P., Stevens, R., Razfar, A. (eds) LOST Opportunities. Explorations of Educational Purpose, vol 23. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4304-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics