Skip to main content

A Role for Fetal Testosterone in Human Sex Differences

Implications for Understanding Autism

  • Chapter
Book cover Autism

Part of the book series: Current Clinical Neurology ((CCNEU))

Abstract

Autism spectrum conditions (ASCs) may be an extreme manifestation of specific male-typical characteristics. Evidence for this theory is provided by the empathizing–systemizing (E–S) theory of sex-typical behavior, which suggests ASCs as an extreme form of the male brain (EMB). In this chapter, we review the evidence supporting EMB theory and examine the effect of hormones on the development of sex differences related to ASCs. An important candidate mechanism for the development of sex-typical behavior is the effect of fetal testosterone (f T) during pregnancy. Evidence that elevated levels of f T may be a risk factor for ASC is also discussed. Many neurodevelopmental conditions occur in males more often than females, including autism, dyslexia, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and early onset persistent antisocial behavior [1]). Autism in particular has been described as an extreme manifestation of some sexually dimorphic traits or an “extreme male brain” [2]. In this chapter, we review the reasons why this condition in particular has been viewed in this light and the evidence related to it.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The American Psychiatric Association uses the term ASD for autism spectrum disorders. We prefer the use of the term ASC as those at the higher functioning end of the autistic spectrum who do not necessarily see themselves as having a “disorder,” and the profile of strengths and difficulties in ASC can be conceptualized as atypical but not necessarily disordered. ASC remains a medical diagnosis, hence the use of the term “condition,” which signals that such individuals need support. We feel that the use of the term ASCs is more respectful to differences; recognizes that the profile in question does not fit a simple “disease” model but includes areas of strength (e.g., in attention to detail) as well as areas of difficulty; and does not identify the individual purely in terms of the latter.

References

  1. Rutter M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: Unifying issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2003;44(8):1092–1015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2002;6(6):248–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. APA. DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A, et al. Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The special needs and autism project (SNAP). Lancet 2006;368(9531):210–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Fombonne E. The changing epidemiology of autism. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2005;18(4):281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Tidmarsh L, Volkmar FR. Diagnosis and epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 2003;48(8):517–525.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bryson SE, Smith IM. Epidemiology of autism: Prevalence, associated characteristics, and implications for research and service delivery. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, Special Issue: Autism 1998;4(2):97–103.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E. Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children: Confirmation of high prevalence. American Journal of Psychiatry 2005;162(6): 1133–1141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Scott F, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton P, Brayne C. Prevalence of autism spectrum conditions in children aged 5–11 years in Cambridgeshire, UK. Autism 2002;6(3):231–237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kraemer S. The fragile male. British Medical Journal 2000;321(7276):1609–1612.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Stodgell CJ, Ingram JI, Hyman SL. The role of candidate genes in unraveling the genetics of autism. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 2001;23:57–81.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Baron-Cohen S. The Essential Difference: Men, Women and the Extreme Male Brain. London: Penguin, 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2004;34(2):163–175.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Baron-Cohen S, Richler J, Bisarya D, Gurunathan N, Wheelwright S. The systemising quotient (SQ): An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 2003;358:361–374.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Goldenfeld N, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. Empathizing and systemizing in males, females and autism. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 2005;2:338–345.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S, Goldenfeld N, et al. Predicting Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) from the Systemizing Quotient-Revised (SQ-R) and Empathy Quotient (EQ). Brain Research 2006;1079(1):47–56.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Samarawickrema N, Atkinson M. The Children’s Empathy Quotient (EQ-C) and Systemizing Quotient (SQ-C): A study of sex differences in typical development and of autism spectrum conditions.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Shah A, Frith U. An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1983;24 (4):613–620.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Jolliffe T, Baron-Cohen S. Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1997;38(5):527–534.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Spong A, Scahill L, Lawson J. Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders 2001;5:47–78.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Lawson J, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2004;34(3):301–310.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lynn R, Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA, Irwing P. Sex differences on the WISC-R in Mauritius. Intelligence 2005;33:527–533.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Allen MH, Lincoln AJ, Kaufman AS. Sequential and simultaneous processing abilities of high-functioning autistic and language-impaired children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1991;21(4):483–502.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Lincoln AJ, Courchesne E, Kilman BA, Elmasian R, Allen M. A study of intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1988;18(4):505–524.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Shah A, Frith C. Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1993;34:1351–1364.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1997;38(7):813–822.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S, Hill J. The Cambridge Mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2006;36(2):169–183.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Scott FJ, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton P, Brayne C. The CAST (Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test): Preliminary development of a UK screen for mainstream primary-school-age children. Autism 2002;6(1):9–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Williams J, Scott F, Stott C, et al. The CAST (Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test): Test accuracy. Autism 2005;9(1):45–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Baron-Cohen S, Scott FJ, Allison C, et al. Estimating autism spectrum prevalence in the population: A school based study from the UK. Submitted.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Williams J, Scott F, Stott C, et al. Sex differences in social and communication skills in primary school aged children.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2001;31:5–17.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Baron-Cohen S, Hoekstra R, Knickmeyer R, Wheelwright S. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) – Adolescent version. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2006;36(3):343–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Allison C. Development of the Autism Spectrum Quotient – Children’s Version (AQ-Child). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Wakabayashi A, Tojo Y, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Japanese version: Evidence from high-functioning clinical group and normal adults. Japanese Journal of Psychology 2004;75(1):78–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Uchiyama T, et al. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) children’s version in Japan: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2006;36:263–270.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Tojo Y. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in Japan: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2006;36(2):263–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hoekstra R, Bartels M, Cath DC, Boomsma DI. Factor structure of the broader autism phenotype and its diagnostic validity: A study using the Dutch translation of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry 2003;60:524–530.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer R, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science 2005;310:819–823.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Baron-Cohen S, Ring H, Chitnis X, et al. fMRI of parents of children with Asperger Syndrome: a pilot study. Brain Cognition 2006;61(1):122–130.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Connellan J, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Batki A, Ahluwalia J. Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior & Development 2000;23(1):113–118.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Baron-Cohen S, Lutchmaya S, Knickmeyer R. Prenatal Testosterone in Mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S. Fetal testosterone and sex differences in typical social development and in Autism. Journal of Child Neurology 2006;21(10):825–845.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Novy M, Resko J (eds). Fetal Endocrinology. New York, New York: Academic Press, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Fuchs F, Klopper A. Endocrinology of Pregnancy. Philadelphia: Harper & Row, 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Tulchinsky D, Little AB. Maternal-Fetal Endocrinology, 2nd ed. Philadelphia, London: W.B. Saunders, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Hines M. Brain Gender. New York, New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Kimura D. Sex and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Cohen-Bendahan CC, van de Beek C, Berenbaum SA. Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: Methods and findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2005;29(2):353–384.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Van de Beek C, Thijssen JHH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Van Goozen SH, Buitelaar JK. Relationships between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord blood: What is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure? Hormones and Behavior 2004;46:663–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Larsen PR, Kronenberg HM, Melmed S, Polonsky KS (eds). Williams Textbook of Endocrinology, 10th ed. Philadelphia: Saunders, 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Jost A. The role of foetal hormones in prenatal development. Harvey Lectures 1961; 55:201–226.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Jost A. Hormonal factors in the sex differentiation of the mammalian foetus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B Biological Sciences 1970; 259(828):119–130.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Jost A. A new look at the mechanism controlling sexual differentiation in mammals. John Hopkins Medical Journal 1972;130:38–53.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. George FW, Wilson JD. Embryology of the Genital Tract. In: Walsh PC, Retik AB, Stamey TA, (eds) Campbell’s Urology, 6th ed., Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1992: 1496–1508.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Collaer ML, Hines M. Human behavioural sex differences: A role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychological Bulletin 1995;118:55–107.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Breedlove SM. Sexual dimorphism in the vertebrate nervous system. The Journal of Neuroscience 1992;12:4133–4142.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Goy RW, McEwen BS. Sexual Differentiation of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  60. New MI. Diagnosis and management of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Annual Review of Medicine 1998;49:311–328.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Hampson E, Rovet JF, Altmann D. Spatial reasoning in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Developmental Neuropsychology 1998; 14(2):299–320.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Hines M, Fane BA, Pasterski VL, Matthews GA, Conway GS, Brook C. Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: Targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28:1010–26.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Resnick SM, Berenbaum SA, Gottesman II, Bouchard TJ. Early hormonal influences on cognitive functioning in congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology 1986;22(2):191–198.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Nass R, Baker S, Speiser P, et al. Hormones and handedness: left-hand bias in female adrenal hyperplasia patients. Neurology 1987;37:711–715.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Berenbaum SA, Hines M. Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science 1992;3:203–206.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Berenbaum SA, Snyder E. Early hormonal influences on childhood sex-typed activity and playmate preferences: Implications for the development of sexual orientation. Developmental Psychology 1995;31:31–42.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Berenbaum SA. Effects of early androgens on sex-typed activities and interests in adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior 1999;35(1):102–110.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Ehrhardt AA, Baker SW. Fetal androgens, human central nervous system differentiation, and behavior sex differences. In: Friedman RC, Richart RR, Vande Wiele RL, (eds) Sex Differences in Behavior. New York: Wiley, 1974:33–51.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Hines M, Brook C, Conway GS. Androgen and psychosexual development: Core gender identity, sexual orientation and recalled childhood gender role behavior in women and men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Journal of Sex Research 2004; 41(1):75–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Knickmeyer RC, Fane BA, Mathews G, et al. Autistic traits in people with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: A test of the fetal testosterone theory of autism. Hormones and Behavior 2006;50(1):148–153.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Fausto-Sterling A. Myths of Gender. New York: Basic Books, 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Quadagno DM, Briscoe R, Quadagno JS. Effects of perinatal gonadal hormones on selected nonsexual behavior patterns: A critical assessment of the nonhuman and human literature. Psychological Bulletin 1977;84:62–80.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Nordenstrom A, Servin A, Bohlin G, Larsson A, Wedell A. Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by the CYP21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 2002;87(11):5119–5124.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Hines M, Ahmed SF, Hughes IA. Psychological outcomes and gender-related development in complete androgen insensitivity syndrome. Archives of Sexual Behavior 2003;32(2):93–101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Money J, Schwartz M, Lewis V. Adult erotosexual status and fetal hormonal masculinization and demasculinization: 46 XX congenital adrenal hyperplasia and 46 XY androgen insensitivity syndrome compared. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1984;9: 405–414.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Hier DB, Crowley WF. Spatial ability in androgen-deficient men. New England Journal of Medicine 1982;306:1202–1205.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  77. Cappa SF, Guariglia C, Papagno C, et al. Patterns of lateralization and peformance levels for verbal and spatial tasks in congenital androgen deficiency. Behavioural Brain Research 1988;31:177–183.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Manning JT, Bundred PE, Flanagan BF. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: A proxy for transactivation activity of the androgen receptor gene? Medical Hypotheses 2002; 59(3):334–336.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT. 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Human Development 2004;77:23–28.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. Brown WM, Hines M, Fane B, Breedlove SM. Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior 2002;42:380–386.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2001;43(3):160–164.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  82. Milne E, White S, Campbell R, Swettenham J, Hansen P, Ramus F. Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2: 4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2006;36(2):225–237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Holt SB. The Genetics of Dermal Ridges. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Kimura D, Carson MW. Dermatoglyphic asymmetry: Relation to sex, handedness and cognitive pattern. Personality and Individual Differences 1995;19(4):471–478.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Kimura D, Clarke PG. Cognitive pattern and dermatoglyphic asymmetry. Personality and Individual Differences 2001;30(4):579–586.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Sanders G, Waters F. Fingerprint asymmetry predicts within sex differences in the performance of sexually dimorphic tasks. Personality and Individual Differences 2001; 31(7):1181–1191.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Walker HA. A dermatoglyphic study of autistic patients. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 1977;7(1):11–21.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. Hartin PJ, Barry RJ. A comparative dermatoglyphic study of autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1979;9(3):233–246.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  89. Hines M, Shipley C. Prenatal exposure to diethylstilbestrol (DES) and the development of sexually dimorphic cognitive abilities and cerebral lateralization. Developmental Psychology 1984;20(1):81–94.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Fein D, Waterhouse L, Lucci D, Pennington B, Humes M. Handedness and cognitve functions in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1985;15:323–333.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. McManus IC, Murray B, Doyle K, Baron-Cohen S. Handedness in childhood autism shows a dissociation of skill and preference. Cortex 1992;28(3):373–381.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  92. Satz P, Soper H, Orsini D, Henry R, Zvi J. Handedness subtypes in autism. Psychiatric Annals 1985;15:447–451.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Soper H, Satz P, Orsini D, Henry R, Zvi J, Schulman M. Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1986;16:155–167.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  94. Peters M. Sex differences in human brain size and the general meaning of differences in brain size. Canadian Journal of Psychology 1991;45(4):507–522.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  95. Gillberg C. Autistic children’s hand preferences: Results from an epidemiological study of infantile autism. Psychiatry Research 1983;10(1):21–30.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  96. Harden AY, Minshew NJ, Mallikarjuhn M, Keshavan MS. Brain volume in autism. Journal of Child Neurology 2001;16(6):421–424.

    Google Scholar 

  97. Tordjman S, Ferrari P, Sulmont V, Duyme M, Roubertoux P. Androgenic activity in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry 1997;154(11):1626–1627.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  98. Ingudomnukul E, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Knickmeyer R. Elevated rates of testosterone-related disorders in women with autism spectrum conditions. Hormones and Behavior 2007;51(5):597–604.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  99. Knickmeyer RC, Wheelwright S, Hoekstra R, Baron-Cohen S. Age of menarche in females with autism spectrum conditions. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2006;48(12):1007–1008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Clark MM, Galef BG. Effects of intraurine position on the behavior and genital morphology of litter-bearing rodents. Developmental Neurology 1998;14:197–211.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Fels E, Bosch LR. Effect of prenatal administration of testosterone on ovarian function in rats. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 1971;111(7):964–969.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  102. Meisel RL, Ward IL. Fetal female rats are masculinized by male littermates located caudally in the uterus. Science 1981;213:239–242.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  103. Even MD, Dhar MG, vom Saal FS. Transport of steroids between fetuses via amniotic fluid in relation to the intrauterine position phenomenon in rats. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 1992;96(2):709–716.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  104. Elizabeth PH, Green R. Childhood sex-role behaviors: Similarities and differences in twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae (Roma) 1984;33(2):173–179.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  105. Miller EM, Martin N. Analysis of the effect of hormones on opposite-sex twin attitudes. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae (Roma) 1995;44(1):41–52.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  106. Resnick SM, Gottesman II, McGue M. Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: an effect of prenatal hormones? Behavior Genetics 1993;23(4):323–329.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  107. Cole-Harding S, Morstad AL, Wilson JR. Spatial ability in members of opposite-sex twin pairs (abstract). Behavior Genetics 1988;18:710.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Rodgers CS, Fagot BI, Winebarger A. Gender-typed toy play in dizygotic twin pairs: A test of hormone transfer theory. Sex Roles 1998;39 (3–4):173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Elkadi S, Nicholls ME, Clode D. Handedness in opposite and same-sex dizygotic twins: Testing and testosterone hypothesis. Neuroreport 1999;10(2):333–336.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  110. Henderson BA, Berenbaum SA. Sex-typed play in opposite-sex twins. Developmental Psychobiology 1997;31:115–123.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  111. Betancur C, Leboyer M, Gillberg C. Increased rate of twins among affected sibling pairs with autism. American Journal of Human Genetics 2002;70(5):1381–1383.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  112. Greenberg DA, Hodge SE, Sowinski J, Nicoll D. Excess of twins among affected sibling pairs with autism: implications for the etiology of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics 2001;69(5):1062–1067.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  113. Croen LA, Grether JK, Selvin S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: Who is at risk? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2002;32(3):217–224.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Hallmayer J, Glasson EJ, Bower C, et al. On the twin risk in autism. American Journal of Human Genetics 2002;71(4):941–946.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Hultman CM, Sparen P, Cnattingius S. Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology 2002;13(4):417–423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology 1959;65:369–382.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  117. Goy RW, Bercovitch FB, McBrair MC. Behavioral masculinization is independent of genital masculinization in prenatally androgenized female rhesus macaques. Hormones and Behavior 1988;22:552–571.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  118. Smail PJ, Reyes FI, Winter JSD, Faiman C. The Fetal Hormonal Environment and its Effect on the Morphogenesis of the Genital System. In: Kogan SJ, Hafez ESE, (eds) Pediatric Andrology. Boston: Martinus Nijhoff, 1981:9–19.

    Google Scholar 

  119. Brown GR, Nevison CM, Fraser HM, Dixson AF. Manipulation of postnatal testosterone levels affect phallic and clitoral development in infant rhesus monkeys. International Journal of Andrology 1999;22:119–128.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  120. Hines M, Golombok S, Rust J, Johnston KJ, Golding J. Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: A longitudinal, population study. Child Development 2002;73(6):1678–1687.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Udry JR, Morris NM, Kovenock J. Androgen effects on women's gendered behaviour. Journal of Biosocial Science 1995;27:359–368.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  122. Udry JR. Biological limits of gender construction. American Sociological Review 2000;65:443–457.

    Google Scholar 

  123. Jacklin CN, Maccoby EE, Doering CH. Neonatal sex-steroid hormones and timidity in 6-18-month-old boys and girls. Developmental Psychobiology 1983;16(3):163–168.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  124. Jacklin CN, Wilcox KT, Maccoby EE. Neonatal sex-steroid hormones and cognitive abilities at six years. Developmental Psychobiology 1988;21(6):567–574.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  125. Marcus J, Maccoby EE, Jacklin CN, Doering CH. Individual differences in mood in early childhood: their relation to gender and neonatal sex steroids. Developmental Psychobiology 1985;18(4):327–340.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  126. Abramovich DR, Rowe P. Foetal plasma testosterone levels at mid-pregnancy and at term: Relationship to foetal sex. Journal of Endocrinology 1973;56(3):621–622.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  127. Forest MG, Sizonenko PC, Cathiard AM, Bertrand J. Hypophyso-gonadal function in humans during the first year of life: I. Evidence for testicular activity in early infancy. The Journal of Clinical Investigation 1974;53:819–828.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  128. Pang S, Levine LS, Chow DM, Faiman C, New MI. Serum androgen concentrations in neonates and young infants with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Clinical Endocrinology 1979;11:575–584.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  129. Dawood MY, Saxena BB. Testosterone and dihydrotestosterone in maternal and cord blood and in amniotic fluid. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 1977; 129:37–42.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  130. Finegan J, Bartleman B, Wong PY. A window for the study of prenatal sex hormone influences on postnatal development. Journal of Genetic Psychology 1989;150(1): 101–112.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  131. Judd HL, Robinson JD, Young PE, Jones OW. Amniotic fluid testosterone levels in midpregnancy. Obstetrics and Gynecolology 1976;48(6):690–692.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  132. Nagamani M, McDonough PG, Ellegood JO, Mahesh VB. Maternal and amniotic fluid steroids throughout human pregnancy. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 1979;134(6):674–680.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  133. Robinson J, Judd H, Young P, Jones D, Yen S. Amniotic fluid androgens and estrogens in midgestation. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology 1977;45:755–761.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  134. Schindler AE. Hormones in human amniotic fluid. Monographs on Endocrinology 1982;21:1–158.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  135. Rodeck CH, Gill D, Rosenberg DA, Collins WP. Testosterone levels in midtrimester maternal and fetal plasma and amniotic fluid. Prenatal Diagnosis 1985;5(3):175–181.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  136. Finegan JK, Niccols GA, Sitarenios G. Relations between prenatal testosterone levels and cognitive abilities at 4 years. Developmental Psychology 1992;28(6):1075–1089.

    Google Scholar 

  137. Grimshaw GM, Sitarenios G, Finegan JK. Mental rotation at 7 years: Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition 1995;29(1):85–100.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  138. Grimshaw GM, Bryden MP, Finegan JK. Relations between prenatal testosterone and cerebral lateralization in children. Neuropsychology 1995;9(1):68–79.

    Google Scholar 

  139. Witelson SF. Sex-differences in neuroanatomical changes with aging. New England Journal of Medical 1991;325(3):211–212.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  140. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S. Fetal testosterone and sex differences. Early Human Development 2006;82(12):755–760.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  141. Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P. Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12 month old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 2002;25:327–335.

    Google Scholar 

  142. Swettenham J, Baron-Cohen S, Charman T, et al. The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and non-social stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1998;9:747–753.

    Google Scholar 

  143. Rutter M. Diagnosis and Definition. In: Schopler IMRE, (ed.) Autism: A Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment. New York: Plenum Press, 1978:1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  144. Hamilton A, Plunkett K, Shafer G. Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British Communicative Inventory: Lower scores in the UK than the USA. Journal of Child Language 2000;27(3):689–705.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  145. Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P. Foetal testosterone and vocabulary size in 18- and 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development 2002;24(4):418–424.

    Google Scholar 

  146. Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy. Hormones and Behavior 2006;49:282–292.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  147. Klin A. Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning Autism and Asperger syndrome: the Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2000;7:831–846.

    Google Scholar 

  148. Bishop DVM. Development of the children's communication checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1998;6:879–891.

    Google Scholar 

  149. Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K. Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2005;46(2):198–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Foetal testosterone and the Child Systemizing Quotient. European Journal of Endocrinology 2006;155(Suppl. 1):S23–S30.

    Google Scholar 

  151. Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test. Social Neuroscience 2006;1:135–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  152. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Chapman E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits. British Journal of Psychology, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  153. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Griffin R, Lawson J, Hill J. The Exact Mind: Empathising and Systemising in Autism Spectrum Conditions. In: Goswami U, (ed.) Handbook of Cognitive Development. Blackwell, 2002.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Humana Press, a part of Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). A Role for Fetal Testosterone in Human Sex Differences. In: Autism. Current Clinical Neurology. Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-60327-489-0_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-60327-489-0_8

  • Publisher Name: Humana Press

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-60327-488-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-60327-489-0

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics