Skip to main content

Creative Insight: The Social Dimension of a Solitary Moment

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

In this chapter we will present a different perspective, by expanding out from this moment in time and embedding it within the other relevant stages of the creative process. When we look at the complete “life span” of a creative insight in our subjects’ experience, the moment of insight appears as but one short flash in a complex, time-consuming, fundamentally social process. It is true that the individuals we interviewed generally report their insights as occurring in solitary moments: during a walk, while taking a shower, or while lying in bed just after waking.

Reproduced with permission from the Nature of Insight, R.J. Sternberg and J. Davidson (Eds.), pp 329−363, Copyright © 1995, MIT Press.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Note the parallels with Mead’s (1934) “generalized other” and with Vygotsky’s (1978) descriptions of how social interactions are internalized to become cognitive processes. Our proposal is consistent with Vygotsky and Mead, particularly the latter’s claim that each distinct social sphere of individual activity will result in a distinct “other”.

  2. 2.

    This anecdote, and those that follow, are based on interviews collected for our ongoing project, Creativity in Later Life, sponsored by the Spencer Foundation.

  3. 3.

    See Martindale (1990, pp. 56–57) for a quick review of theories that include such a dichotomy, starting with Nietzsche’s well-known distinction between Apollonian and Dionysian creativity.

References

  • Barron, F. (1988). Putting creativity to work. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 76–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in scientific discovery. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the holistic approach to behavior. In K. S. Pope & J. S. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A systems view of creativity. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 325–339). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990a). The domain of creativity. In: M.A. Runco, R.S. Albert (eds.) Theories of creativity (pp. 190–212). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990b). Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1990). Wisdom: an evolutionary interpretation. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed) The nature of wisdom. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D.C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R. (1990). Generativity theory and creativity. In: M.A. Runco, R.S. Albert (eds.) Theories of creativity (pp. 116–140). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M.W. (1982). Attention and arousal. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, D.H. (1988). Creativity: dreams, insights, and transformations. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 271–297). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J.W. (1964). Creative thinking, problem-solving, and instruction. In: E.R. Hilgard (ed.) Theories of learning and instruction (sixty-third yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education) (pp. 240–267). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • GetzeIs, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghiselin, B. (1952). The creative process. New York: Mentor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H.E. (1981). Darwin on man: a study of scientific creativity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1974).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H.E., & Davis, S.N. (1988). Inching our way up Mount Olympus: the evolving-system approach to creative thinking. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 243–270). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadarnard, J. (1949). The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, D.M. (1990) The ecology of human creativity. A psychological perspective. In: M.A. Runco & R.S. Albert (eds.) Theories of creativity (pp. 143–169). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, S. M. (1990). Problem finding/solving in science: Moving toward theory. Creativity Res J, 3(4), 330–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • John-Steiner, V. (1992). Creative lives, creative tensions. Creativity Res J, 5(1), 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, P., & Jones, R. (1988). A computational model of scientific insight. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 177–201). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1990). The clockwork muse: the predictability of artistic change. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, R.M. (1990). Creativity. An idea whose time has come and gone? In: M.A. Runco, & R.S. Albert (eds.) Theories of creativity (pp. 215–233). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1985). The society of mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (1984). Restructuring revisited: an information processing theory of restructuring and insight. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 25, 117–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Omstein, R. (1986). Multimind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D.N. (1988). The possibility of invention. In: R.J. Sternberg (ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 362–385). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poincaré, H. (1913). The foundations of science. New York: The Science Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, R. A. (1990). Implicit theories and ideational creativity. In M. A. Runco & R. S. Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp. 234–252). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shrady, M. (1972). Moments of insight. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. (1977). Boston studies in the philosophy of science: Vol. 54. Models of discovery. Boston: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1988a). Scientific genius: a psychology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1988b). Creativity, leadership, and chance. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 386–426). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1988). A three-facet model of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 125–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E.P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing, ln R.J. Steinberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 43–75). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodman RW, Schoenfeldt LF (1989) Individual differences in creativity: an interactionist perspective. In: Glover JA, Ronning RR, Reynolds CR (eds) Handbook of creativity. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by a grant from the Spencer Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Csikszentmihalyi, M., Sawyer, K. (2014). Creative Insight: The Social Dimension of a Solitary Moment. In: The Systems Model of Creativity. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9085-7_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics