Skip to main content

Evidence of Dynamic Changes in Brain Processing from Imaging Techniques: Implications for Interventions for Developmental Disabilities

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 6))

Abstract

There is great interest in emerging literacy skills due to evidence that such skills identified in the preschool period are predictive of reading at school age. Further, there is evidence that interventions applied early in the reading acquisition process have more positive outcomes than those applied after poor reading performance has been identified as delayed. To better understand the development of emergent and conventional reading skills, more research is needed to expand the evidence of how brain processing changes are related reading skill development and to changes due to intervention. This chapter reviews research on the critical emergent literacy skills that underlie the development of early reading skills, and links between brain processing of one of these critical emergent skills (speech processing) in infants, children and adults to language and reading skills. The chapter concludes with a dynamic model of brain activity as it relates to the acquisition of reading-related skills and to the impact of educational interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ackerman, P. T., & Dykman, R. A. (1993). Phonological processes, confrontational naming, and immediate memory in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 597–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print: A summary. Champaign-Urbana: Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews-Espy, K., Molfese, D., Molfese, V., & Modglin, A. (2004). Development of auditory event-related potentials in young children and relations to word-level reading abilities at 8 years. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 9–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. (1995). Predicting reading ability over the long term: The changing roles of letter naming, phonological awareness and orthographic processing. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 29–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, E., & Blachman, B. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blatchford, P., Burke, J., Farquhar, C., Plewis, I., & Tizard, B. (1987). Associations between pre-school reading related skills and later reading achievement. British Educational Research Journal, 13(1), 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borello, U., & Pierani, A. (2010). Patterning the cerebral cortex: Traveling with morphogens. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 20, 408–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 476–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. (2001). Nonword repetition and children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S., Fowler, A., Stone, B., & Winbury, N. (1994). Training phonological awareness: A study with inner-city kindergarten children. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, I., & Felton, R. (1990). Effects of instruction on beginning reading skills in children at risk for reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 223–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., MacLean, M., Bradley, L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme, alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 313–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burhanpurkar, A., & Barron, R. (2001, April). Origins of phonological awareness skill in pre-readers: Roles of language, memory and proto-literacy. Poster presented at Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, H. A., & Christie, B. R. (2007). Do new neurons have a functional role in the adult hippocampus? Debates in Neuroscience, 1, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J., Snowling, M., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913–923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Does phonological awareness help children learn to read? Cognition, 91, 77–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. (1991). Early identification of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow-up study of speech-language impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1987). Learning to be learning disabled. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 22, 155–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Chisum, H. J., Townsend, J., Cowles, A., Covington, J., Egaas, B., Harwood, M., Hinds, S., & Press, G. A. (2000). Normal brain development and aging: Quantitative analysis at in vivo MR imaging in healthy volunteers. Radiology, 216, 672–682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. (1990). Explicit versus implicit instruction in phoneme awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 429–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuppini, R., Bucherelli, C., et al. (2006). Age-related naturally occurring depression of hippocampal neurogenesis does not affect trace fear conditioning. Hippocampus, 16, 141–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeJong, P., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeJong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 22–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children’s reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. Washington, DC: U.S Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dufva, M., Niemi, P., & Voeten, M. (2001). The role of phonological memory, word recognition and comprehension skills in reading development: From preschool to grade 2. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 91–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1983). A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In L. Gentile, M. Kamil, & J. Blanchard (Eds.), Reading research revisited (pp. 143–153). Columbus: Charles E. Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J., Foorman, B., Shaywitz, S., & Shaywitz, B. (1999). Conceptual and methodological issues in dyslexia research: A lesson for developmental disorders. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonaryova, A. P., Dove, G. O., & Maguire, M. J. (2005). Linking brainwaves to the brain: An ERP primer. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 183–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. R., Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1997). The case for early reading intervention. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia. Implications for early intervention (pp. 243–264). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent, T. F., III, Herman, D. H., Clasen, L. S., Toga, A. W., Rapoport, J. L., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). From the cover: Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(21), 8174–8179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, C. S. (1996). Mechanisms and molecules that control growth cone guidance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 341–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: An updated on genes, brains and environments. The Journal of Child Psychiatry, 42, 91–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttorm, T. K., Leppðnen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A-M., Eklund, K. M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Brain event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex, 41, 291–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. (1949). The organization of behavior; A Neuropsychological Theory. New York: Wiley-Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2000). Two alternative ways to model the relation between reading accuracy and phonological awareness at preschool age. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 77–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, P. R. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R., & Watson, J. (1997). Developing reading, spelling and phonemic awareness skills in primary school children. Reading, 44, 37–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Key, A. P., Molfese, D. L., & Ratajczak, E. D. (2006). ERP indicators of learning in adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 379–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, N., McGee, T., Micco, A., Sharma, A., Carrell, T., & Nicol, T. (1993). Mismatch negativity in school-age children to speech stimuli that are just perceptually different. Journal of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials, 88, 123–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C., Burgess, S., Anthony, J., & Barker, T. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, L. K. and Cheng, H. J. (2006). Axon pruning: an essential step underlying the developmental plasticity of neuronal connections. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361, 1531–1544

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T. K., Laakso, M.-L., Leinonen, S., Leppanen, P. H. T., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Puolakanaho, A., Richardson, U., & Viholainen, H. (2001). Developmental pathways of children with and without familial risk for dyslexia during the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 539–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Leppänen, P. H. T., Richardson, U., & Guttorm, T. (2003). In V. Csepe (Ed.), Dyslexia: Different brain, different behaviour (Neuropsychology and cognition series, pp. 113–152). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., Kulju, P., Laakso, M. L., Leiwo, M., Leppänen, P., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A. M., Richardson, U., Torppa, M., & Viholainen, H. (2004a). Early development of children at familial risk for dyslexia—Follow-up from birth to school age. Dyslexia, 10, 146–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Erskine, J., Guttorm, T. K., Laakso, M. L., Leppanen, P. H. T., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A. M., Richardson, U., & Torppa, M. (2004b). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to school age. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 185–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Guttorm, T. K., Huttunen, T., Hämäläinen, J., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Vesterinen, M. (2005). Psychophysiology of developmental dyslexia: A review of findings including studies of children at risk for dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 167–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Ronimus, M., Alanko, A., Poikkeus, A. M., & Taanila, M. (2007). Early identification of dyslexia and the use of computer game-based practice to support reading acquisition. Nordic Psychology, 59, 109–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marín, O., Valiente, M., Ge, X., & Tsai, L.-H. (2010). Guiding neuronal cell migrations. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, 2, a001834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1996). Models of speech perception and phonological processing in reading. Child Development, 67, 1836–1856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Manis, F. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 323–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuinness, D. (2004). Early reading instruction: What science really tells us about how to teaching reading. Cambridge, MA: A Bradford Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuinness, D., McGuinness, C., & Donohue, J. (1995). Phonological training and the alphabet principle: Evidence for reciprocal causality. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 830–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuinness, C., McGuinness, D., & McGuinness, G. (1996). PhonoGraphix: A new method for remediating reading difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 73–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. (1978a). Electrophysiological correlates of categorical speech perception in adults. Brain and Language, 5, 25–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. (1978b). Left and right hemisphere involvement in speech perception: Electrophysiological correlates. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 237–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. (1995). Electrophysiological responses obtained during infancy and their relation to later language development: Further findings. In M. G. Tramontana & S. R. Hooper (Eds.), Advances in child neuropsychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1–11). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. (1998). Electrophysiological correlates of early speech perception and language development during infancy and early childhood. In N. Raz (Ed.), Insights into neural foundations of cognition from life span development perspective. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72, 238–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Hess, T. (1978). Speech perception in nursery school age children: Sex and hemisphere differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 71–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1979a). Hemisphere and stimulus differences as reflected in the cortical responses of newborn infants to speech stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 15, 505–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1979b). Infant speech perception: Learned or innate? In H. Witaker & H. Witaker (Eds.), Advances in neurolinguistics (Vol. 4). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1980). Cortical responses of preterm infants to phonetic and nonphonetic speech stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 16, 574–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1985). Electrophysiological indices of auditory discrimination in newborn infants: The bases for predicting later language development. Infant Behavior & Development, 8, 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1988). Right hemisphere responses from preschool children to temporal cues contained in speech and nonspeech materials: Electrophysiological correlates. Brain and Language, 33, 245–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, D. L. (1997a). The use of brain recordings to assess learning. Proceedings of the International Conference on Engineering Education: Progress through partnerships (Vol. 2). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., & Molfese, V. (1997b). Discrimination of language skills at five years of age using event-related potentials recorded at birth. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 135–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (2000). The continuum of language development during infancy and early childhood: Electrophysiological correlates. In C. Rovee-Collier, L. P. Lipsitt, & H. Hayne (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 12). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (2001, October). Invited keynote address. Longitudinal studies of reading-related learning disabilities: Age-to-age relationships among neurocognitive, familial and academic variables. The Annual Conference of the International Dyslexia Association. Albuquerque, NM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., Morse, P. A., & Peters, C. J. (1990). Auditory evoked responses from infants to names for different objects: Cross modal processing as a basis for early language acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 26, 780–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, V., Molfese, D., Modglin, A., Walker, J., & Neamon, J. (2004). Screening early reading skills in preschool children: Get ready to read. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 22, 136–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, V., Modglin, A., Beswick, J., Neamon, J., Berg, S., Berg, J., & Molnar, A. (2006). Letter knowledge, phonological processing and print awareness: Skill development in non-reading preschool children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 296–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., Molfese, V., & Molfese, P. (2007a). Relation between early measures of brain responses to language stimuli and childhood performance on behavioral language tasks. In D. Coch, G. Dawson, & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain, Second edition: Atypical development. New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., Molfese, V. J., & Pratt, N. L. (2007b). The use of event-related evoked potentials to predict developmental outcomes. In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-related potentials. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D., Molfese, V., Beswick, J., Jacobi-Vessels, J., Molfese, P., & Key, A. F. (2008). Dynamic links between emerging cognitive skills and brain processes. Special Issue on Neurobiological and experiential dimensions of dyslexia: Multiple perspectives. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 682–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V. (1994). Influence of phonological awareness and letter knowledge on beginning reading and spelling development. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Reading development and dyslexia. San Diego: Singular Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51, 187–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the national early literacy panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. www.nifl.gov/earlychildhood/NELP/NELPreport.html

  • Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816–833.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puolakanaho, A., Poikkeus, A.-M., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). Assessment of three-and-a-half-year-old children’s emerging phonological awareness in a computer animation context. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 416–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puolakanaho, A., Poikkeus, A.-M., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Emerging phonological awareness as a precursor of risk in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 221–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichert, H. (2009). Evolutionary conservation of mechanisms for neural regionalization, proliferation and interconnection in brain development. Biological Letters, 5, 112–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Roth, E., & Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills in letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 284–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for general causal inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shors, T. J., Townsend, D. A., Zhao, M., Kozorovitskiy, Y., & Gould, E. (2002). Neurogenesis may relate to some but not all types of hippocampal-dependent learning. Hippocampus, 12, 578–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C., Burns, M., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M. (1999). Getting ready for reading: Early phonemic awareness and phonics teaching improves reading and spelling in inner-city second language learners. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 587–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teilhard de Chardin, P. (1955). The phenomenon of man. New York: Harper & Row Publishers, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 148–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Rashotte, C., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). The contribution of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word reading skills in second to fifth grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Rashotte, C., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., & Garvan, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses in instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (1998). Language prediction skill, phonological recoding ability, and beginning reading. In C. H. Hulme & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 33–47). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E. R., Small, S. G., Pratt, A., Chen, R. S., & Denckla, M. B. (1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult to remediate and readily remediated poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 601–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F., Fletcher, J., Snowling, M., & Scanlon, D. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., Hecht, S., Barker, T., Burgess, S., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, J., Denton, K., & Germino-Hausken, E. (2000). America’s kindergartners. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. NCES 2000–070. Retrieved March 8, 2004, from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2000070

  • Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (2001). Get ready to read! Columbus: Pearson Early Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988–1000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01-HD17860, R01 DC005994), and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (5R01HL070911).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dennis L. Molfese Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Molfese, D.L., Molfese, V.J., Garrod, K., Molfese, D.L. (2012). Evidence of Dynamic Changes in Brain Processing from Imaging Techniques: Implications for Interventions for Developmental Disabilities. In: Breznitz, Z., Rubinsten, O., Molfese, V., Molfese, D. (eds) Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices. Literacy Studies, vol 6. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4086-0_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics