Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT,volume 84))

  • 484 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter investigates instances of NI in which the order V+N is found. Languages with V+N order constitute apparent counter-examples to the proposal put forth here. What I will show is that the IN in some of the languages under consideration is larger than in instances of prototypical NI. This will be obvious in the Niuean examples discussed below, but not for Mapudungun and Upriver Halkomelem, where the IN genuinely appears bare. In all cases, however, I show that asymmetric c-command holds between the verbal root and the IN, thus allowing the LCA to establish a V+N order for the NI constructions here.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Citing Karetu (1974: 97).

  2. 2.

    Citing Jones and Biggs (1995).

  3. 3.

    There are two kinds of pseudo noun incorporation in Niuean . While it is true that in general incorporation, indicative relatives are impossible, they are possible in fai-incorporation structures (Massam 2009). Fai is a verb which roughly means “to have”, thus, fai-incorporation structures indicate possession, thus asserting the existence of the object in question.

  4. 4.

    The reader will note that there is no categorizing nP projection. These are not discussed in Kahnemuyipour and Massam and are left out of the discussion here. For the current purposes, I assume that an N projection corresponds to an nP in which the root has raised to SpecnP as seen in many of the other languages presented here.

  5. 5.

    The tacit assumption here is that K has an uninterpretable Case feature, thus only KPs need to raise to a position in the clause to check Case. Recall that Massam derives VSO word order by remnant movement of the VP to an EPP position in the IP domain, thus requiring overt movement of the object KP to a higher position.

  6. 6.

    A related question here is whether the IN has the same kind of word-level stress as non-incorporated nominals, since I propose that a phasal n is present (see the discussion at the beginning of Chapter 5 and on Romance compounds in the next section).

  7. 7.

    A crucial piece of data that is missing from Donohue (1999) is whether the perfective marker can appear after a full DP object. This, of course, makes strong predictions regarding the surface position of the incorporated object and the full DP object, but these questions cannot be answered here.

  8. 8.

    N+V compounding is by-and-large absent in Romance. One notable exception is Catalan , which has semi-productive N+V NI (Gracia and Fullana 1999). Unlike the IN in V+N compounding constructions discussed here for the rest of Romance, the IN in N+V NI constructions in Catalan is a bare noun without number marking.

  9. 9.

    This is akin to Newell’s proposal where she explicitly distinguishes between vP (which introduces the external argument) and vP (which is the true verbalizer, such as -ify). See also Harley (2007) for related discussion.

  10. 10.

    One might think that an unaccusative n is not a phase head, given Chomsky’s (2001; 2008) arguments that passive and unaccusative v is not a strong phase. See Legate (2003), however, for arguments that passive and unaccusative v does constitute a strong phase.

  11. 11.

    This of course only accounts for the uniform gender of these compounds within a single language. It does not account for the fact that these compounds are masculine in all the Romance languages examined here, nor is it intended to. Only a historical study into compounds in Proto-Romance can answer this question.

  12. 12.

    One unanswered question is whether the √P raises to SpecnP given that there are otherwise overt nominalizers in the language. Given the arguments throughout this monograph, the answer would seem to be yes, although I have not explored the ramifications of this putative movement.

  13. 13.

    Baker et al. (2005: 169) show clearly that Mapudungun has true possessor stranding rather than applied objects as Michelson (1991) argued for Oneida .

  14. 14.

    This kind of leapfrogging movement was argued for in Koopman and Szabolcsi (2000) for the formation of the verbal complex in Hungarian based on similar evidence. Specifically, the anti-mirror effects (in the sense of Baker 1985), in which V+N order is found, in inconsistent with a pure head raising analysis. Why some projections undergo Complement-to-Spec roll-up and others do not is a mystery which will have to await future research.

  15. 15.

    Lexical suffixes are wide-spread across the Salish family. See the references cited for more sources on other Salish languages.

  16. 16.

    This contradicts the findings of Gerdts (2003), where she presents data that lexical suffixation does affect argument structure. This may be due to dialectal differences between Upriver Halkomelem and Island Halkomelem (from which Gerdts draws her observations).

  17. 17.

    I have represented the empty DP argument as pro; however it remains to be seen what the nature of this empty category is.

  18. 18.

    Recall that I am assuming a single-engine hypothesis for syntax and morphology (Julien 2002; Marantz 1997). Regardless, it is clear from the examples that a more than a single projection is needed for the incorporated noun in Onondaga since nominalizing morphology is typically assumed to head its own projection (Marantz 2001; Ogawa 2001).

  19. 19.

    See, for instance, Cinque (1999) or Alexiadou (1997) who claim that adverbs can appear as specifiers of functional projections or as heads either of the same projection (as Cinque discusses) or as non-projecting heads that are sister to V (as Alexiadou discusses). Also, Wiltschko (2004) proposes that a given syntactic item can either project (as a head) or not (as an adjunct), from one language to the next.

References

  • Alexiadou, Artemis. 1997. Adverb Placement: A Case Study in Antisymmetric Syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark C. 1985. “The Mirror Principle and Morphosyntactic Explanation.” Linguistic Inquiry 16 (3):373–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark C. 1996. The Polysynthesis Parameter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark C., Roberto Aranovich, and Lucía A. Golluscio. 2005. “Two Types of Syntactic Noun Incorporation: Noun Incorporation in Mapudungun and Its Typological Implications.” Language 81 (1):138–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, Douglas. 2005. “Phrasal Noun Incorporation in Tongan.” In AFLA XII, edited by Jeffrey Heinz, and Dimitris Ntelitheos: UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brousseau, Anne-Marie, and Elena Voskovskaia. 2007. “Nominalisation furtive dans les composés synthétiques: où se cache l’affixe-zéro?” Paper presented at the 11th Bilingual Workshop on Theoretical Linguistics, Concordia University, December 7–8, 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. “Derivation by Phase.” In Ken Hale: A Life in Language, edited by Michael Kenstowicz, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2008. “On Phases.” In Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory, edited by P. Oltero, 133–66. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra, and William Ladusaw. 2004. Restriction and Saturation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1999. Adverbs and Functional Heads: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Sciullo, Anna Maria, and Edwin Williams. 1987. On the Definition of Word. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donohue, Mark. 1999. A Grammar of Tukang Besi. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fradin, Bernard. 2009. “IE, Romance: French.” In The Oxford Handbook of Compounding, edited by Rochelle Lieber, and Pavol Stekauer, 417–35. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galloway, Brent. 1980. The Structure of Upriver Halkomelem, a Grammatical Sketch and Classified Word List for Upriver Halkomelem. Sardis, BC: Coqualeetza Education Training Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galloway, Brent. 1993. A Grammar of Upriver Halkomelem. Berkeley, Los Angeles, London: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerdts, Donna B. 2003. “The Morphosyntax of Halkomelem Lexical Suffixes.” International Journal of American Linguistics 69 (4):345–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gracia, Luisa, and Olga Fullana. 1999. “On Catalan Verbal Compounds.” Probus 11 (2):239–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, Heidi. 2007. External Arguments: On the Independence of Voice° and v°. Tromsø, Norway, April 12–14, 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, Heidi. 2009. “Compounding in Distributed Morphology.” In The Oxford Handbook of Compounding, edited by Rochelle Lieber, and Pavol Štekauer, 129–44. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmelink, Bryan L. 1992. “La incorporación Nominal en el Mapudungun.” Lenguas Modernas 19:129–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, Pei Te Hurinui, and Bruce Biggs. 1995. Nga iwi o Tainui: The Traditional History of the Tainui People. Auckland, New Zealand: Auckland University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julien, Marit. 2002. Syntactic Heads and Word Formation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahnemuyipour, Arsalan. 2009. The Syntax of Sentential Stress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kahnemuyipour, Arsalan, and Diane Massam. 2003. “Inversely Ordered DPs in Niuean.” In The Proceedings of the 2002 annual meeting of the Canadian Linguistics Association, edited by Sophie Burelle, and Stanca Somesfalean, 156–66. Université du Québec.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahnemuyipour, Arsalan, and Diane Massam. 2006. “Patterns of Phrasal Movement: The Niuean DP.” In Clause Structure and Adjuncts in Austronesian Languages, edited by Hans-Martin Gaertner, Paul Law, and Joachim Sabel, 125–50. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Karetu, T. S. 1974. Te Reo Rangatira: A Course in Māori for Sixth and Seventh Forms. Wellington, New Zealand: GP Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koopman, Hilda, and Anna Szabolcsi. 2000. Verbal Complexes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornfeld, Laura Malena. 2009. “IE, Romance: Spanish.” In The Oxford Handbook of Compounding, edited by Rochelle Lieber, and Pavol Stekauer, 436–52. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krämer, Martin. 2009. The Phonology of Italian. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. “Severing the External Argument from its Verb.” In Phrase Structure and the Lexicon, edited by Johan Rooryck, and Laurie Zaring, 109–38. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legate, Julie. 2003. “Some Interface Properties of the Phase.” Linguistic Inquiry 34 (3):506–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieber, Rochelle. 1992. Deconstructing Morphology. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 1997. “No Escape from Syntax: Don’t Try Morphological Analysis in the Privacy of Your Own Lexicon.” University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2):201–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 2001. “Words.” Paper presented at the 20th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, University of Southern California, February 23–25, 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marvin, Tatjana. 2003. Topics in the Stress and Syntax of Words. PhD diss., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massam, Diane. 2000. “Niuean Nominalization.” In Proceedings of AFLA 7, edited by Marian Klamer. Amsterdam: Vrije Universiteit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massam, Diane. 2001. “Pseudo Noun Incorporation in Niuean.” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 19 (1):153–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massam, Diane. 2009. “Existential Incorporation Constructions.” Lingua 119 (2):166–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Megerdoomian, Karine. 2008. “Parallel Nominal and Verbal Projections.” In Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, edited by Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero, and Maria Luisa Zubizaretta, 73–103. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelson, Karin. 1991. “Possessor Stranding in Oneida.” Linguistic Inquiry 22 (4):756–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelson, Karin, and Mercy Doxtator. 2002. Oneida-English/English-Oneida Dictionary. Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nespor, Marina. 1999. “Stress Domains.” In Word Prosodic Systems in Languages of Europe, edited by Harry Van der Hulst, 117–60. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, Heather. 2008. “Aspects of the Morphology and Phonology of Phases.” PhD diss., McGill University, Montreal, QC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa, Yoshiki. 2001. A Unified Theory of Verbal and Nominal Projections. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1982. The Syntax of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, Ineke. 2008. A Grammar of Mapuche. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ulfsbjorninn, Shanti. 2009. “The Internal Structure of Compounds: A Phase Account of Aphasia.” Iberia 1 (1):143–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiltschko, Martina. 2004. “Expletive Categorical Information: A Case Study of Number Marking in Halkomelem Salish.” In Northeast Linguistics Society (NELS). Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiltschko, Martina. 2009. “√Root Incorporation: Evidence from Lexical Suffixes in Halkomelem Salish.” Lingua 119 (2):199–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Barrie .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Barrie, M. (2011). V+N Order. In: Dynamic Antisymmetry and the Syntax of Noun Incorporation. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol 84. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1570-7_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics