Skip to main content

A Cultural Psychological Promise to the Study of Religiosity: Background and Context of the “Dialogical Self”

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Towards Cultural Psychology of Religion
  • 885 Accesses

Abstract

An obvious conclusion to be derived from the previous chapter could be formulated thus: When we, like Wundt, understand religion as an element of culture, we need concepts and units of analysis that will enable us to investigate the nexus between a certain culture (or cultural context) and the person. Wundt himself did not yet dispose of them; but they have now become available with such concepts as activity, action, habitus, and also narrative or “story.” The theorizing about the “dialogical self” as initiated by Hermans and Kempen may count as an example as well. Their work – which has been well received by the international cultural psychology “movement” (Hermans 1999a, b, 2001a, b, 2002, 2003; Hermans and Dimaggio 2007; Valsiner 2001) – is promising for a cultural psychological analysis of religion. It is particularly interesting to take a closer look at this body of theory, as its development sustains historical relationships with the psychology of religion. The dialogical self may be regarded as a belated result of a much older Dutch initiative to integrate cultural psychology and psychology of religion, which led to the establishment of a department for the psychology of culture and religion at Nijmegen, in the Netherlands, in 1956 (although the roots of the initiative reach back to the founding Years of psychology in general in the Netherlands). As well as this, it catches up with a stand that has become fundamental to all psychologies of religion for a long time: psychological research on religion must be performed from a secular perspective (cf. Belzen 2001c). To corroborate these claims, it is necessary to draw substantially on historical information as well as on information about recent developments. I shall therefore present a mixture of historical and systematic argumentation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    To be historically correct and to use the proper terminology: Roels started out as an assistant to C. Winkler (1855–1941), Professor of Psychiatry and Neurology at Utrecht University. In 1916 he became a private teacher at the Faculty of Humanities and Philosophy. He was appointed “lecturer” at Utrecht in 1918, and “professor” in 1922. Since 1980, the lectureships at Dutch universities are called professorates. Neither function nor title exist anymore.

  2. 2.

    In 1975, when at his former institute operationalism and testing of hypotheses had become dominant too, Rutten published a short article, in which he recommends psychologists to read “great literature” (philosophers, poets, novelists). He wrote that as psychologists, we “… are captured in a certain historical way of thinking. The professional Language we have been taught, and the methods and techniques that we have learned to handle, orient the way in which we perceive behavior. There is real danger that the expertise we have gained will hinder new developments. We are constantly forced to struggle to get hold of and to overcome the doctrinary conformism required by any training” (Rutten 1975, p. 391).

  3. 3.

    In his text on cultural psychology, some ten Years later, Roels devotes only a small remark to religion: “A beginning has hardly been made with a psychology of worldviews, that is, a psychology that explores the psychic-spiritual structures or inner attitudes, from which different types of worldviews stem” (1928, p. 88). This sounds rather casual, unlike his earlier plea for the subdiscipline. But one cannot infer from this quotation that he had abandoned the apologetic aims he pursued with the psychology of religion: The 1919 text was addressed to a Roman Catholic audience; and the passage from 1928 comes from a publication by the Psychological Laboratory at the religiously neutral State University of Utrecht. To what “psychology of worldviews” Roels is referring here remains unclear. Perhaps to Jaspers (1922)?

  4. 4.

    It is possible of course that they had read this work, or that Roels had become aware of this part of Wundt’s theorizing via his teacher Michotte in Leuven, who had been working at the Leipzig laboratory in 1906, precisely at the time when Wundt was working on his cultural psychology, which he considered a natural complement to his earlier, experimental work in psychology. According to Wundt, psychology would have to be plural. Psychology can only turn to experiment as an auxiliary method if it seeks to examine the “elementary psychic processes”; but if it seeks to study the higher psychic processes it has to consult other sciences for orientation (Wundt 1900–1909). Wundt’s own suggestion was that psychologists should consult history (see Chapters 2 and Chapter 7).

  5. 5.

    Of course, Roman Catholic influences on the psychology at Nijmegen can also be detected in the strong interest in phenomenological psychology, a psychology that tried to be not natural-scientific in nature, and therefore according to many Catholic psychologists, would be more apt for investigating the psyche. Relations between Catholicism and phenomenology can be clearly pointed out in the work of such well-known Nijmegen professors as Strasser and Buytendijk.

  6. 6.

    For a correct understanding, one should differentiate clearly the employment of the theorizing about the dialogical self from the employment of just the Self-Confrontation Method as developed by Hermans earlier. The use of the Self-Confrontation Method as such does not make an investigation a psychological one (it may be used in theological research too, cf. Putman 1988), and is not necessarily associated with a cultural psychological perspective. Research from the perspective of the dialogical self, however, will always also be an exploration of a subject’s personal culture, as I hope to show more concretely in an empirical study reported in Chapter 11.

References

  • Abma, R. (1983). “Methodisch zonder confessie.” Uit de geschiedenis van de Nijmeegse psychologie. [Methodical without confession: From the history of the Nijmegen psychology] Nijmegen: Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen, Psychologisch Laboratorium (internal report).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1929/1973). Problems of Dostoevsky’s poetics. (2nd ed.) Ann Arbor, MI: Ardis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belzen, J. A. (2001c). The introduction of psychology of religion to the Netherlands: Ambivalent reception, epistemological concerns, and persistent patterns. Journal for the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 37, 45–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belzen, J. A. (2002b). Die Gleichzeitigkeit des Ungleichen: Anmerkungen zur Entwicklung der Religionspsychologie im niederländischen Sprachraum. [The simultaneity of the unequal: Comments on the development of the psychology of religion in the Dutch speaking world] In Chr. Henning & E. Nestler (Eds), Konversion: Zur Aktualität eines Jahrhundertthemas [Conversion: Topicality of a centenarian theme] (pp. 117–144). Frankfurt: Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belzen, J. A. (2007). Psychologie en het raadsel van de religie: Beschouwingen bij een eeuw godsdienstpsychologie in Nederland. [Psychology and the mystery of religion: Reflections on a century of psychology of religion in the Netherlands] Amsterdam: Boom.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belzen, J. A. (Ed.) (2009b). Ideology, politics and personality: Shaping forces in Dutch psychology of religion, 1907–1957. History of Psychology, 12, paginanummers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its Language. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortmann, H. M. M. (1964–1968). Als ziende de Onzienlijke: Een cultuurpsychologische studie over de religieuze waarneming en de zogenaamde religieuze projectie. [Seeing the Unseeable: A cultural-psychological study on religious perception and the so-called religious projection] (4 vols.) Hilversum: Brand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortmann, H. M. M. (1968). Hindoes en boeddhisten: Dagboekaantekeningen en reisbrieven. [Hindus and Buddhists: Diary notes and journey letters] Baarn: Ambo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Girgensohn, K. (1921/1930). Der Seelische Aufbau des Religiösen Erlebens: Eine religionspsychologische Untersuchung auf Experimenteller Grundlage. [The psychological structure of religious experiences: A religious-psychological investigation based on experimental research] Gütersloh: Bertelsmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grünbaum, A. A. (1928). Het ik-bewustzijn en de psychische ontwikkeling. [Self-consciousness and psychological development] Utrecht: s.n.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1967). Motivatie en prestatie. [Motivation and performance] Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1970). A questionnaire measure of achievement motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 353–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1971). Prestatiemotief en faalangst in gezin en onderwijs, tevens handleiding bij de Prestatie Motivatie Test voor Kinderen (PMT-K). [Motivation of performance and fear of failure: Manual to the Performance Motivation Test for Children] Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1974). Waardegebieden en hun ontwikkeling. [Fields of positioning and their development] Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1981). Persoonlijkheid en waardering. Deel 1: Organisatie en opbouw der waarderingen. [Personality and positioning. Part 1: Organization and construction of positions] Lisse: Swets en Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1999a). Dialogical thinking and self-innovation. Culture & Psychology, 5, 67–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (1999b). The innovative potentials of agreement and disagreement in dialogical history. Culture & Psychology, 5, 491–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (2001a). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology, 7, 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (2001b). The construction of a personal position reportoire: Method and practice. Culture & Psychology, 7, 323–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (2002). Special issue on the Dialogical Self. Theory & Psychology, 12 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (2003). Special issue on the Dialogical Self. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 16 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. & Dimaggio, G. (2007). Self, identity, and globalization in times of uncertainty: A dialogical analysis. Review of General Psychology, 11, 31–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. & Hermans-Jansen, E. (1995). Self-narratives: The construction of meaning in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. & Hermans-Jansen, E. (2003). Dialogical processes and the development of the self. In J. Valsiner & K. L. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 534–559). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. & Kempen, H. J. G. (1993). The dialogical self: Meaning as movement. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. & Kempen, H. J. G. (1998). Moving cultures: The perilous problem of cultural dichotomies in a globalizing society. American Psychologist, 53, 1111–1120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M., Kempen, H. J. G. & Loon, R. J. P. van (1992). The dialogical self: Beyond individualism and rationalism. American Psychologist, 47, 23–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M., Rijks, T. I. & Kempen, H. J. G. (1993). Imaginal dialogues of the self: Theory and method. Journal of Personality, 61 (2), 207–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, H. (1997). Psychological concepts, their products and consumers. Culture & Psychology, 3, 115–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. London: MacMillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jaspers, K. (1922). Psychologie der Weltanschauungen. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1999). Personal narratives and the life story. In L. A. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.; pp. 478–500). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roels, F. J. M. A. (1918). De toekomst der psychologie. [The future of psychology] Den Bosch: Teulings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roels, F. J. M. A. (1919–1920). Godsdienstpsychologie en apologetiek. [Psychology of religion and apologetics] De Beiaard, [The Carillon] 4 (2), 337–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roels, F. J. M. A. (1928). Cultuurpsychologie en psychotechniek. [Psychology of culture and psychotechnics] Mededeelingen van het psychologisch laboratorium R.U.Utrecht, [Announcements of the Psychological Laboratory University of Utrecht] 77–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutten, F. J. Th. (1937). Het domein der godsdienstpsychologie. [The field of the psychology of religion] Nijmegen: Centrale Drukkerij.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutten, F. J. Th. (1947). De overgang van het agrarische volkstype in het industriële. [The transition of the agricultural to the industrial type] Amsterdam: Koninklijke Nederlandse Academie van Wetenschappen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutten, F. J. Th. (1954). Verschil tussen de Amerikaanse en Europese benadering der psychologische problemen. [Difference between the American and European approach to psychological problems] De Tijd, [The Times] June 17, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutten, F. J. Th. (1975). Een lijstje titels uit de wereldliteratuur. [A list of titles from world literature] Gedrag, [Behavior] 6, 391–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarbin, T. R. (Ed.) (1986a). Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (1917). Die menschliche Persönlichkeit. [The human personality] Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suèr, H. (1969). Niet te geloven: De geschiedenis van een pastorale commissie. [Don’t you believe it: The history of a pastoral committee] Bussum: Paul Brand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2001). The first six Years: Culture’s adventure in psychology. Culture & Psychology, 7, 5–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. & VanderVeer, R. (2000). The social mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westhoff, H. (1996). Geestelijke bevrijders: Nederlandse katholieken en hun beweging voor geestelijke volksgezondheid in de twintigste eeuw. [Spiritual redemptors: Dutch Catholics and their movement for mental public health in the 20th century] Nijmegen: Valkhof.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1900–1909). Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythos und Sitte. [Folk psychology: An investigation of the laws of development of Language, mythe and mores] Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1915). Völkerpsychologie. Vol. 6: Mythos und Religion [Myth and religion] (3rd part). Leipzig: Kröner.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jacob A. Belzen .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Belzen, J.A. (2010). A Cultural Psychological Promise to the Study of Religiosity: Background and Context of the “Dialogical Self”. In: Towards Cultural Psychology of Religion. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3491-5_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics