Skip to main content

Looking at Dogs: Moving from Anthropocentrism to Canid Umwelt

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

As a companion to humans, the domestic dog is naturally interpreted from a human-centered (anthropocentric) perspective. Indeed, dog behavior and actions are often explained by using anthropomorphisms: attributions to the dog that would hold if the actor were human. While sometimes useful, anthropomorphisms also have the potential to be misleading or incorrect. In this chapter we describe work to replace an anthropocentric perspective with a more dog-centered research program. First we detail research systematically testing anthropomorphisms of emotional complexity—the appearance of guilt and jealousy—that are made of dogs, by testing the context of appearance of the “guilty look” and by testing advantageous and disadvantageous inequity aversion. Relatedly, we describe research looking at the contribution of specific dog physical attributes to human preference and anthropomorphizing. Finally, we identify anthropocentric and canid-centric elements of our own and others’ research, and suggest ways that research can be more sensitive to the dog’s umwelt.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Disobedience was assured by the treat being offered to the dog by the experimenter after the owner left the room, and obedience, by the treat's immediate removal. In pilot trials, no difference was seen in the dogs' behavior whether they ate the treat because it was provided by the experimenter or of their own accord.

  2. 2.

    Putting aside, of course, the many studies of dogs as tracking dogs or explosives-, drug-, or disease-detection dogs. These research programs are highly relevant for an understanding not just of these working dogs, but also of the entire species. However, these programs do not arise from the cognitive and behavioral fields with which this chapter is concerned.

References

  • American Kennel Club. (2013). AKC meet the dogs: Great Pyrenees. Retrieved February 15, 2013, http://www.akc.org/breeds/index.cfm

  • Bauer, E. B., & Smuts, B. B. (2007). Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 73, 489–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (1972). The development of social interaction, play, and meta-communication in mammals: An ethological perspective. Quarterly Review of Biology, 47, 412–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (1974). Social play in coyotes, wolves, and dogs. BioScience, 24, 225–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2004). Wild justice and fair play: Cooperation, forgiveness, and morality in animals. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 489–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Social Justice Research, 19, 153–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2013) Justice- and fairness-related behaviors in nonhuman primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 10416–10423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D. & Tomasello, M. (2013). Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? Animal Cognition, 16, 137–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buttner, A. P., & Strasser, R. (2014). Contagious yawning, social cognition, and arousal: An investigation of the processes underlying shelter dogs’ responses to human yawns. Animal Cognition, 17, 95–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Craven, B. A., Paterson, E. G., & Settles, G. S. (2010). The fluid dynamics of canine olfaction: Unique nasal airflow patterns as an explanation of macrosmia. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 7, 933–943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custance, D., & Mayer, J. (2012) Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: An exploratory study. Animal Cognition, 15, 851–859.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872/1979). The expression of emotions in man and animals. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeKay, M. L., & McClelland, G. H. (1996). Probability and utility of endangered species preservation programs. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 2, 60–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (1997). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranton, C., Rödel, H. G., Bedossa, T., & Belkhir, S. (2013). Inverse sex differences on performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in a repeated problem solving task. Poster presented at the International Ethological Conference, Newcastle, U.K.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Povinelli, D. J. (1993). Attribution of cognitive states to animals: Anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 49, 87–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagen, R. (1981). Animal play behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, M. W. (1978). The dog: Its domestication and behavior. New York: Garland STPM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fratkin, J. L., & Baker, S. C. (2013). The role of coat color and ear shape on the perception of personality in dogs. Anthrozoös, 26, 125–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi, M., McGreevy, P., Kara, E., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). Effects of selection for cooperation and attention in dogs. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon, S., & Doré, F. Y. (1992). Search behavior in various breeds of adult dogs (Canis familiaris): Object permanence and olfactory cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. G. Jr. (l970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazit, I., & Terkel, J. (2003). Domination of olfaction over vision in explosives detection by dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 82, 65–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenebaum, J. (2004). It’s a dog’s life: Elevating status from pet to “fur baby” at Yappy Hour. Society and Animals, 12, 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harr, A., Gilbert, V., & Phillips, K. (2009). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) show contagious yawning? Animal Cognition, 12, 833–837.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Comins, J. A., Pytka, L. M., Cahill, D. P., & Velez-Calderon, S. (2011). What experimental experience affects dogs’ comprehension of human communicative actions? Behavioural Processes, 86, 7–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1946). Emotion in man and animal: An analysis of the intuitive process of recognition. Psychological Review, 53, 88–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hecht, J., & Horowitz, A. (2013). Physical prompts to anthropomorphism of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 8, e30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hecht, J., Miklósi, Á., & Gácsi, M. (2012). Behavioral assessment and owner perceptions of behaviors associated with guilt in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 139, 134–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemsworth, P. H. (2003). Human-animal interactions in livestock production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 81, 185–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2009a). Disambiguating the “guilty look”: Salient prompts to a familiar dog behavior. Behavioural Processes, 81, 447–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2009b). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Animal Cognition, 12, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2009c). Inside of a dog. New York: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2012). Fair is fine, but more is better: Limits to inequity aversion in the domestic dog. Social Justice Research, 25, 195–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. C., & Bekoff, M. (2007). Naturalizing anthropomorphism: Behavioral prompts to our humanizing of animals. Anthrozoös, 20(1), 23–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A., Hecht, J., & Dedrick, A. (2013). Smelling more or less: Investigating the olfactory experience of the domestic dog. Learning and Motivation, 44, 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joly-Mascheroni, R., Senju, A., & Shepherd, A. (2008). Dogs catch human yawns. Biology Letters, 4, 446–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Pitsch, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Dogs steal in the dark. Animal Cognition, 16, 385–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaulfuß, P., & Mills, D. S. (2008). Neophilia in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and its implication for studies of dog cognition. Animal Cognition, 11, 553–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellert, S. R. (1996). The value of life: Biological diversity and human society. Washington, DC: Island Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Kaplan, F., Gácsi, M., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2004). Social behaviour of dogs encountering AIBO, an animal-like robot in a neutral and in a feeding situation. Behavioural Processes, 65, 231–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kundey, S. M. A., De Los Reyes, A., Taglang, C., Allen, R., Molina, S., Royer, E.et al. (2010). Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) react to what others can and cannot hear. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 126, 45–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonardi, R. J., Vick, S-J., & Dufour, V. (2012). Waiting for more: The performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks. Animal Cognition, 15, 107–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, S. R. (2000). Handbook of applied dog behavior and training (Vol 1). Ames, Iowa: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1950/1971): Part and parcel in animal and human societies. In K. Lorenz (Ed.), Studies in animal and human behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 115–195). London: Methuen

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1954). Man meets dog. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W. A. (2006). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) seek help in an emergency? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 113–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, P. B. (2006). For the love of a dog: Understanding emotion in you and your best friend. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch, M., Jezierski, T., Broffman, M., Hubbard, A., Turner, K., & Janecki, T. (2006). Diagnostic accuracy of canine scent detection in early- and late-stage lung and breast cancers. Integrative Cancer Therapies, 5, 30–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGreevy, P., Grassia, T. D., & Harmanb, A. M. (2004). A strong correlation exists between the distribution of retinal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63, 13–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mech, D. (1970). The wolf: The ecology and behaviour of an endangered species. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, Á. (2007). Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. E., & Murphey, C. J. (1995). Vision in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 207, 1623–1634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millot, J. L. (1994). Olfactory and visual cues in the interaction systems between dogs and children. Behavioural Processes, 33, 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R .W., & Hamm, M. (1996). The interpretation of animal psychology: Anthropomorphism or behavior reading? Behaviour, 134, 173–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art, religion and science. London: Thames and Hudson Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morewedge, C. K., Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, P. H., Doe, C., & Godsell, E. (2008). Secondary emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports and subjective claims by animal owners. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 3–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, P., Fidler, M., & Costall, A. (2000). Beyond anecdotes: An empirical study of “anthropomorphism.” Society and Animals, 8, 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naderi, S., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V. (2001). Co-operative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 74, 59–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pongrácz, P., Vida, V., Bánhegyi, P., & Miklósi, Á. (2008). How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? Animal Cognition, 11, 75–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prato-Previde, E., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Valsecchi, P. (2008). Is your choice my choice? The owners’ effect on pet dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) performance in a food choice task. Animal Cognition, 11, 167–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, D., Dunnette, M. D., & Jorgenson, D. O. (1972). Effects of perceptions of equity and inequity on worker performance and satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 75–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Range, F., Horn, L., Viranyi, Z. & Huber, L. (2009). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 340–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98, 5937–3942.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivas, J. A., & Burghardt, G. M. (2002). Crotalomorphism: A metaphor to understand anthropomorphism by omission. In M. Bekoff, A. Colin, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 9–17). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1883). Mental evolution in animals. London: Keegan Paul, Trench & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romero, T., Konno, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2013). Familiarity bias and physiological responses in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy. PLoS ONE 8(8), e71365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rooney, N. J., Bradshaw, J. W. S., & Robinson, I. R. (2000). A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 66, 235–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rugaas, T. (2006) On talking terms with dogs: Calming signals. Wenatchee, WA: Dogwise.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, C. R. (1993). Understanding dogs: Caretakers’ attributes of mindedness in canine-human relationships. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 22, 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, C. R. (2003). Actions speak louder than words: Close relationships between humans and nonhuman animals. Symbolic Interaction, 26, 405–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheider, L., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a command? Animal Cognition, 16, 361–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenkel, R. (1967). Submission: Its features and function in the wolf and dog. American Zoologist, 7, 319–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serpell, J. A. (2003). Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection—beyond the “cute response.” Society & Animals, 11, 83–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Settles, G. S., Kester, D. A., & Dodson-Dreibelbis, L. J. (2003). The external aerodynamics of canine olfaction. In F. G. Barth, J. A. C. Humphrey, & T. W. Secomb (Eds.), Sensors and sensing in biology and engineering (pp. 323–355). New York: SpringerWein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sillero-Zubiri, C., Hoffmann, M., & Macdonald, D. D. W. (2004) Canids: Foxes, wolves, jackals and dogs: Status survey and conservation action plan. Gland, CH: IUCN/SSC Canid Specialist Group

    Google Scholar 

  • Silva, K., Bessa, J., & de Sousa, L. (2012). Auditory contagious yawning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): First evidence for social modulation. Animal Cognition, 15, 721–724.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slabbert, J. M., Rasa, O., & Anne, E. (1997). Observational learning of an acquired maternal behavior pattern by working dog pups: An alternative training method? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 53, 309–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2002). Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 27–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szetei, V., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2003). When dogs seem to lose their nose: An investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 83, 141–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tapp, P., Siwak, C., Estrada, J., Holowachuk, D., & Migram, N.W. (2003). Effects of age on measures of complex working memory span in the beagle dog (Canis familiaris) using two versions of a spatial list learning paradigm. Learning & Memory, 10, 148–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tembrock, G. (1976). Canid vocalizations. Behavioural Processes, 1, 57–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., Miklósi, A., & Csányi, V. (1997). Dog–human relationship affects problem solving behaviour in dogs. Anthrozoös, 10, 214–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (1934/1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept (pp. 5–80). New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C., & Smuts, B. B. (2007). Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Animal Cognition, 10, 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, E., Miller, K., Mohan-Gibbons, H., & Vela, C. (2012). Why did you choose this pet?: Adopters and pet selection preferences in five animal shelters in the united states. Animals, 2, 144–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F., Hunter, E. A., Mendl, M. T., & Lawrence, A. B. (2000). The spontaneous qualitative assessment of behavioural expressions in pigs: First explorations of a novel methodology for integrative animal welfare measurement. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 67, 193–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, R. E., & Young, R. J. (2002). Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? Animal Cognition, 5, 183–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitely, H .E. (2006). Understanding and training your dog or puppy. Santa Fe, NM: Sunstone Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne, C. D. L. (2007). What are animals? Why anthropomorphism is still not a scientific approach to behavior. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 2, 125–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin, S. (2002). A new perspective on barking in dogs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 189–193.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the members of the Horowitz Dog Cognition Lab who helped to run the experiments mentioned herein, and to Animal Haven, a non-profit shelter in New York City that generously donated use of their facility for running trials. We are indebted to the dog owners who participate in our experiments, and doubly so to their dogs.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexandra Horowitz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Horowitz, A., Hecht, J. (2014). Looking at Dogs: Moving from Anthropocentrism to Canid Umwelt . In: Horowitz, A. (eds) Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53994-7_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics