Skip to main content

Neurobiology: Language By, In, Through and Across the Brain

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 2349 Accesses

Part of the book series: The Frontiers Collection ((FRONTCOLL))

Abstract

Language has come to be commonly understood as something accomplished by the brain. Much scientific investigation has consequently focused in looking for language in the brain. Although it may sound intuitive, this approach suggests that language is an object located inside another object. This spatial metaphor has generated important insights into the brain sites important for language, from nineteenth century studies of brain-damaged patients to more recent and refined evidence from brain imaging.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Chomsky’s forays into dualistic rationalism were blended with and at least partly motivated by his political views, which eclectically (and bizarrely) equated empiricism with US imperialism of the Vietnam era and rationalism with progressive enlightenment (Chomsky 1972b).

  2. 2.

    Interestingly, this scientific debate had political and ideological undertones in mid-nineteenth century France of the second empire, as holistic views were considered a conservative and theologically warranted reflection of the unity of the soul, whereas localizationism was associated with antimonarchic and anticlerical left-wing views at the time (Harrington 1987).

  3. 3.

    Modern neurology has provided evidence in support of diaschisis. For example, in patients with damage in cerebral cortex, reduced glucose metabolism is often seen far away from the site of damage, in the opposite hemisphere of the cerebellum. This phenomenon, called ‘crossed cerebellar diaschisis’, is attributed to contralateral (“crossed”) connections between forebrain and cerebellum (Gold and Lauritzen 2002; Pantano et al. 1986).

  4. 4.

    In more technical verbatim terms: Broca’s area “handles only intrasentential dependency relations” (Grodzinsky 2000: 21) and “is the neural home to receptive mechanisms involved in the computation of the relation between transformationally moved phrasal constituents and their extraction sites...” (ibid.: 1).

  5. 5.

    Corresponding issues have been debated in artificial intelligence for decades. Computational models informed by cognitive theories may approximate the behavior of a human being, but their internal organization in no way resembles functional brain organization. The cognitive model-driven approach in AI (e.g., Newell and Simon 1963) was countered in the 1980s by parallel-distributed (“connectionist”) models (McClelland and Rumelhart 1986). These were inspired ‘bottom-up’ by simplified features of neurons and their connections and aspired to demonstrate complex input-output behaviors mimicking human cognition as emergent properties of such very simple neural networks.

  6. 6.

    Since conventional fMRI statistics test for activation effects separately in each voxel, the large number of voxels creates a need to correct for multiple comparisons. For example, a given voxel shows an effect that is significant at the 95 % confidence level (i.e., the probability of obtaining the finding by chance alone is \(\le 5\,\%\)). If comparisons are performed in 10 voxels, the probability of obtaining the finding by chance increases to 50 %. A correction for multiple comparisons is therefore necessary. In its simplest form (the “Bonferroni correction”), the p-value is multiplied by the number of comparisons. Such a correction would require that an effect in a single voxel reaches a p-value of 0.0000005 in order to “survive” a Bonferroni correction for 100,000 comparisons (assuming that an fMRI study includes this number of brain voxels).

  7. 7.

    In technical terms, the region of interest approach drastically reduces the need to correct for multiple comparisons, as only a small part of the brain is considered, thus increasing the probability of a statistically significant finding.

  8. 8.

    Among these are statistical power (number of participants), analysis type (whole-brain vs. region of interest), signal to noise ratio (reflecting head motion and other sources of noise), and the investigator’s selection of significance thresholds and method of multiple comparison correction.

  9. 9.

    Participants had to judge whether sentences were the same or different. For some sentences, this required syntactic processing (e.g., “The policeman arrested the thief” is the same as “The thief was arrested by the policeman”), whereas in other sentence pairs it required semantic processing (“East of the city is the lake” is different from “East of the city is the river”).

  10. 10.

    Function words (e.g., “if” or “by”) are primarily grammatical with relatively little meaning, whereas content words (e.g., nouns such as “house” or “love”) are meaningful (semantically rich).

  11. 11.

    Garden-path sentences have misleading syntactic structure. A classic example is the sentence “The horse raced past the barn fell”, where the final word prompts reanalysis of the initially expected syntactic structure (“The horse raced past the barn”).

  12. 12.

    Churchland’s eliminative materialism claims that psychology will be eventually replaced by neuroscience (1986).

  13. 13.

    The grammatical structure of sentences is not fully determined by word order, but also by insertion of small and relatively meaningless grammatical words (such as prepositions), changes in word endings etc. This is reflected in the term “morphosyntax”. For example, interpretation of the sentence “The Rottweiler was bitten by the mailman” is not possible based on word order alone, but requires decoding of morphological cues (passive voice indicated by the auxiliary verb “be” and the ending of the verb “bite”, which makes it a participle) and the preposition “by” indicating that “mailman” is the agent of the sentence, i.e. the one who (unexpectedly) does the biting.

  14. 14.

    A minicolumn includes about 100 neurons with strong excitatory interconnections. These neurons are vertically arranged, giving the impression of a ‘column’. Minicolumns are considered basic functional units of mammalian cortex (Mountcastle 1997).

  15. 15.

    For an example, consider the length of the vowel /i/ in the word “fit”, which is around 60 ms. With length of > 100 ms, a phonemic boundary will be crossed and the vowel will be perceived as a long /i:/, resulting in the word “feet”. On the other hand, a change in vowel length from 100 to 150 ms will not be perceived as a different phoneme (and the perceived word will remain “feet”). Whereas acoustic length of the vowel can thus vary in graded fashion, some relatively small changes in vowel length around the category boundary will be perceived as distinct phonemes that can change the meaning of a word.

  16. 16.

    The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an EEG response that follows about 150–250 ms after a deviant stimulus (e.g., a high tone among a series of low tones, or an omitted click in an otherwise regular sequence of clicks; Cheour et al. 2000). It can be reliably measured even in infants and therefore provides a technique for demonstrating perceptual discrimination at an age when verbal responses cannot be obtained. If an infant shows an MMN to stimulus A in a series of presentations of stimulus B, one can infer that the infant brain is able to distinguish A from B.

  17. 17.

    Greenough et al. (1987, p. 540) define experience-expectant plasticity as “designed to utilize the sort of environmental information that is ubiquitous and has been so throughout much of the evolutionary history of a species.” An example is the development of ocular dominance columns, which are small vertically organized units in visual cortex that respond to information coming from one eye only.

  18. 18.

    For a more comprehensive review of the literature on developmental change in language networks see Müller and Palmer (2008).

  19. 19.

    Meyer and colleagues (2000) instructed some of their participants to repair sentence violations. For example, when presented with the sentence “The spy was in the caught”, participants would not only indicate that the sentence was ungrammatical, but also covertly fix the violation, generating “The spy was caught” in their minds (without speaking). Activation for this repair condition was particularly strong in right IFG and STG.

  20. 20.

    Unlike English, German morphosyntax (cf. Footnote 13) relies heavily on grammatical gender since every noun has one of three genders (feminine, masculine, neuter) and articles and pronouns change in agreement with the gender of the corresponding noun. For example, in “Die Tüte riss und ihr Inhalt fiel heraus” (“The bag tore and its contents fell down”), the article “die” and the pronoun “ihr” have the feminine form, in agreement with the grammatical gender of “Tüte”.

  21. 21.

    Donald Hebb (1949) hypothesized that a synapse is strengthened as a result of synchronous firing of the two neurons the synapse connects. This idea, popularized in the phrase “What fires together wires together”, has proven basically accurate.

References

  • Abeles, M.: Corticonics. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1991)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackermann, H.: Cerebellar contributions to speech production and speech perception: psycholinguistic and neurobiological perspectives. Trends Neurosci. 31, 265–272 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., Zilles, K.: Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 206–215 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anwander, A., Tittgemeyer, M., von Cramon, D.Y., Friederici, A.D., Knosche, T.R.: Connectivity-based parcellation of Broca’s area. Cereb. Cortex 17, 816–825 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aydin, K., Ciftci, K., Terzibasioglu, E., Ozkan, M., Demirtas, A., Sencer, S., Minareci, O.: Quantitative proton MR spectroscopic findings of cortical reorganization in the auditory cortex of musicians. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 26, 128–136 (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R.I., Friederici, A.D.: Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca’s area. Neuroimage 42, 525–534 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillieux, H., De Smet, H.J., Paquier, P.F., De Deyn, P.P., Marien, P.: Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 110, 763–773 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, D.F., Ardila, A.: Aphasia. Oxford University Press, New York (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Binkofski, F., Amunts, K., Stephan, K.M., Posse, S., Schormann, T., Freund, H.J., Zilles, K., Seitz, R.J.: Broca’s region subserves imagery of motion: a combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 273–285 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J.R., Burman, D.D., Meyer, J.R., Gitelman, D.R., Parrish, T.B., Mesulam, M.M.: Modality independence of word comprehension. Hum. Brain Mapp. 16, 251–261 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J.R., Burman, D.D., Meyer, J.R., Gitelman, D.R., Parrish, T.B., Mesulam, M.M.: Development of brain mechanisms for processing orthographic and phonologic representations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1234–1249 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bortfeld, H., Wruck, E., Boas, D.A.: Assessing infants’ cortical response to speech using near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 34, 407–415 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brauer, J., Friederici, A.D.: Functional neural networks of semantic and syntactic processes in the developing brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1609–1623 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois, J.P., Synaptogenesis in the neocortex of the newborn: The ultimate frontier for individuation? In: Nelson, C.A., Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Cognitive neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge (MA), pp. 23–34 (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg, V., Schüz, A., Anatomy of Cortex. Statistics and Geometry. Springer, Berlin (1991)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brauer, J., Neumann, J., Friederici, A.D.: Temporal dynamics of perisylvian activation during language processing in children and adults. Neuroimage 41, 1484–1492 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broca, P.: Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de la parole). Bulletins et Mémoires de la Société Anatomique de Paris 36, 330–357 (1861)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J.W.: Introduction: Microgenetic theory. In: Brown, J.W. (ed.) The Life of the Mind, pp. 1–26. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T.T., Lugar, H.M., Coalson, R.S., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, S.E., Schlaggar, B.L.: Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cereb. Cortex 15, 275–290 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Riggio, L.: The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain Lang. 89, 370–376 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., Waters, D.: Sign language and the brain: a review. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 13, 3–20 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canolty, R.T., Soltani, M., Dalal, S.S., Edwards, E., Dronkers, N.F., Nagarajan, S.S., Kirsch, H.E., Barbaro, N.M., Knight, R.T.: Spatiotemporal dynamics of word processing in the human brain. Front. Neurosci. 1, 185–196 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G.: Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 541–552 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G.: PET studies of syntactic processing with auditory sentence presentation. Neuroimage 9, 343–351 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catani, M., Jones, D.K., ffytche, D.H.: Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann. Neurol. 57, 8–16 (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, C.D., Garavan, H., Bellgrove, M.A.: Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 631–646 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K., Naatanen, R.: Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 351–353 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheour, M., Leppanen, P.H., Kraus, N.: Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 4–16 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheour, M., Martynova, O., Naatanen, R., Erkkola, R., Sillanpaa, M., Kero, P., Raz, A., Kaipio, M.L., Hiltunen, J., Aaltonen, O., Savela, J., Hamalainen, H.: Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature 415, 599–600 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Syntactic Structures. Mouton, s’-Gravenhage (1957)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Review of B.F. Skinner "Verbal Behavior". Language 35, 26–58 (1959)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge (1965)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Cartesian Linguistics. Harper & Row, New York (1966)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Language and Mind. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York (1972a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Psychology and ideology. Cognition 1, 11–46 (1972b)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Reflections on Language. Temple Smith, London (1976)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N.: Rules and Representations. Columbia University Press, New York (1980)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Ronat, M.: Language and Responsibility. Pantheon, Hassocks (1979)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chugani, H.T., Phelps, M.E., Mazziotta, J.C.: Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann. Neurol. 22, 487–497 (1987)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P.S.: Neurophilosophy. MIT Press, Cambridge (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, E., Dewhurst, K.: An Illustrated History of Brain Function. Sanford, Oxford (1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Salmon, E.: The functional anatomy of inhibition processes investigated with the Hayling task. Neuroimage 14, 258–267 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., Hertz-Pannier, L.: Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science 298, 2013–2015 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hertz-Pannier, L., Dubois, J., Meriaux, S., Roche, A., Sigman, M., Dehaene, S.: Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 103, 14240–14245 (2006)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Devauchelle, A.D., Oppenheim, C., Rizzi, L., Dehaene, S., Pallier, C.: Sentence syntax and content in the human temporal lobe: an fMRI adaptation study in auditory and visual modalities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1000–1012 (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dronkers, N.F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M.T., Cabanis, E.A.: Paul Broca’s historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain 130, 1432–1441 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffau, H.: The anatomo-functional connectivity of language revisited. New insights provided by electrostimulation and tractography. Neuropsychologia 46, 927–934 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., Owen, A.M.: Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., Taub, E.: Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270, 305–307 (1995)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebach, C.J., Schlesewsky, M., Lohmann, G., Von Cramon, D.Y., Friederici, A.D.: Revisiting the role of Broca’s area in sentence processing: Syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 24, 79–91 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiez, J.A., Raichle, M.E., Tallal, P., Petersen, S.E.: Activation of left frontal area near Broca’s area during auditory detection and phonological access tasks. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 13(Suppl. 1), S504 (1993)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincham, J.M., Carter, C.S., van Veen, V., Stenger, V.A., Anderson, J.R.: Neural mechanisms of planning: a computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 99, 3346–3351 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Finney, E.M., Fine, I., Dobkins, K.R.: Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1171–1173 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S.E., Marcus, G.F.: The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nat. Rev. Genet. 7, 9–20 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A.: The Modularity of Mind. MIT Press, Cambridge (1983)

    Google Scholar 

  • Forman, S.D., Cohen, J.D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W.F., Mintun, M.A., Noll, D.C.: Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Kotz, S.A.: The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional imaging and lesion studies. Neuroimage 20(Suppl 1), S8–17 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Opitz, B., von Cramon, D.Y.: Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: an fMRI investigation of different word types. Cereb. Cortex 10, 698–705 (2000a)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Wang, Y., Herrmann, C.S., Maess, B., Oertel, U.: Localization of early syntactic processes in frontal and temporal cortical areas: a magnetoencephalographic study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 1–11 (2000b)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Ruschemeyer, S.A., Hahne, A., Fiebach, C.J.: The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb. Cortex 13, 170–177 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A.D., Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J.: The role of the posterior superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension. Neuroreport 20, 563–568 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H.: The Mind’s New Science, 2nd edn. Basic Books, New York (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N.: The organization of language and the brain. Science 170, 940–944 (1970)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Gold, L., Lauritzen, M.: Neuronal deactivation explains decreased cerebellar blood flow in response to focal cerebral ischemia or suppressed neocortical function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 7699–7704 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K.: Language and Language Disturbances. Grune & Stratton, New York (1948)

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M.A., Westwood, D.A.: An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 203–211 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenough, W.T., Black, J.E., Bailey, C.H.: Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 58, 539–559 (1987)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grill-Spector, K., Malach, R.: The human visual cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 649–677 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y.: Theoretical Perspectives on Language Deficits. MIT Press, Cambridge (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y.: The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca’s area. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 1–71 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guzzetta, A., Pecini, C., Biagi, L., Tosetti, M., Brizzolara, D., Chilosi, A., Cipriani, P., Petacchi, E., Cioni, G.: Language organisation in left perinatal stroke. Neuropediatrics 39, 157–163 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, M., Peterson, B.S., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J.C., Gore, J.C.: Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 247–262 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamzei, F., Rijntjes, M., Dettmers, C., Glauche, V., Weiller, C., Buchel, C.: The human action recognition system and its relationship to Broca’s area: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 19, 637–644 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Härle, M., Dobe, C., Cohen, R., Rockstroh, B.: Brain activity during syntactic and semantic processing-a magnetoencephalographic study. Brain Topogr. 15, 3–11 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, A.: Medicine, Mind, and the Double Brain. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslinger, B., Erhard, P., Weilke, F., Ceballos-Baumann, A.O., Bartenstein, P., Grafin von Einsiedel, H., Schwaiger, M., Conrad, B., Boecker, H.: The role of lateral premotor-cerebellar-parietal circuits in motor sequence control: a parametric fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 159–168 (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Head, H.: Studies in Neurology. Oxford University Press, London (1920)

    Google Scholar 

  • Head, H.: Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1926)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O.: The Organization of Behavior. John Wiley & Sons, New York (1949)

    Google Scholar 

  • Heim, S., Opitz, B., Friederici, A.D.: Distributed cortical networks for syntax processing: Broca’s area as the common denominator. Brain Lang. 85, 402–408 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hein, G., Knight, R.T.: Superior temporal sulcus-It’s my area: or is it? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 2125–2136 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., Mazziotta, J.C.: The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1123–1128 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herholz, K., Thiel, A., Wienhard, K., Pietrzyk, U., von Stockhausen, H.-M., Karbe, H., Kessler, J., Bruckbauer, T., Halber, M., Heiss, W.D.: Individual functional anatomy of verb generation. Neuroimage 3, 185–194 (1996)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, S.K., Plante, E., Weber Byars, A., Strawsburg, R.H., Schmithorst, V.J., Ball Jr, W.S.: Normal fMRI brain activation patterns in children performing a verb generation task. Neuroimage 14, 837–843 (2001)

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, S.K., Vannest, J., Mecoli, M., Jacola, L.M., Tillema, J.M., Karunanayaka, P.R., Schmithorst, V.J., Yuan, W., Plante, E., Byars, A.W.: Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. Int. J. Audiol. 46, 533–551 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Homae, F., Hashimoto, R., Nakajima, K., Miyashita, Y., Sakai, K.L.: From perception to sentence comprehension: the convergence of auditory and visual information of language in the left inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage 16, 883–900 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughlings-Jackson, J.: On affections of speech from disease of the brain. Brain 1, 304–330 (1878)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hund-Georgiadis, M., von Cramon, D.Y.: Motor-learning-related changes in piano players and non-musicians revealed by functional magnetic-resonance signals. Exp. Brain Res. 125, 417–425 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hwang, K., Palmer, E.D., Basho, S., Zadra, J.R., Müller, R.-A.: Category specific activations during word generation reflect experiential sensorimotor modalities. Neuroimage 48, 717–725 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, K.L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A.C., Schlaug, G.: Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R.A., Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.: Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 95, 8410–8413 (1998)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M.A., Carpenter, P.A., Keller, T.A., Eddy, W.F., Thulborn, K.R.: Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274, 114–116 (1996)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Justus, T.: The cerebellum and English grammatical morphology: evidence from production, comprehension, and grammaticality judgments. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1115–1130 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E.R., Jessell, T.M., Sanes, J.R.: Sensory experience and the fine tuning of synaptic connections. In: Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., Jessell, T.M. (eds.) Principles of Neural Science, pp. 1115–1130. Elsevier, New York (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, T.A., Carpenter, P.A., Just, M.A.: The neural bases of sentence comprehension: a fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cereb. Cortex 11, 223–237 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knösche, T.R., Maess, B., Friederici, A.D.: Processing of syntactic information monitored by brain surface current density mapping based on MEG. Brain Topogr. 12, 75–87 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Kameyama, M., Nakahara, K., Sekihara, K., Miyashita, Y.: Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 80–84 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krams, M., Rushworth, M.F., Deiber, M.P., Frackowiak, R.S., Passingham, R.E.: The preparation, execution and suppression of copied movements in the human brain. Exp. Brain Res. 120, 386–398 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota, M., Inouchi, M., Ferrari, P., Roberts, T.P.: Human magnetoencephalographic evidence of early syntactic responses to c-selection violations of English infinitives and gerunds by L1 and L2 speakers. Neurosci. Lett. 384, 300–304 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperberg, G.R., Holcomb, P.J., Sitnikova, T., Greve, D., Dale, A.M., Caplan, D.: Distinct patterns of neural modulation during the processing of conceptual and syntactic anomalies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 272–293 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., Hillyard, S.A.: Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 307, 161–163 (1984)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, H., Kuriki, S., Kim, J.M., Lee, Y.H., Kim, K., Nam, K.: MEG study on neural activities associated with syntactic and semantic violations in spoken Korean sentences. Neurosci. Res. 51, 349–357 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A.D., Ulbert, I., Schroeder, C.E.: Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110–113 (2008)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Le Bihan, D.: Looking into the functional architecture of the brain with diffusion MRI. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 469–480 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E.H.: Biological Foundations of Language. Wiley, New York (1967)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lidzba, K., Staudt, M., Wilke, M., Grodd, W., Krageloh-Mann, I.: Lesion-induced right-hemispheric language and organization of nonverbal functions. Neuroreport 17, 929–933 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liégeois, F., Connelly, A., Baldeweg, T., Vargha-Khadem, F.: Speaking with a single cerebral hemisphere: fMRI language organization after hemispherectomy in childhood. Brain Lang. 106, 195–203 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maess, B., Herrmann, C.S., Hahne, A., Nakamura, A., Friederici, A.D.: Localizing the distributed language network responsible for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence processing. Brain Res. 1096, 163–172 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marie, P.: Revision de la question de l’aphasie. La Semaine Médicale 26, 241–247 (1906)

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A.: The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 25–45 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J., Rumelhart, D.E. (eds.): Parallel Distributed Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge (Mass.) (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., Rogers, T.T.: The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 310–322 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M., Friederici, A.D., von Cramon, D.Y.: Neurocognition of auditory sentence comprehension: event related fMRI reveals sensitivity to syntactic violations and task demands. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 19–33 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E.K., Cohen, J.D.: An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, D.L., Coffey-Corina, S.A., Neville, H.J.: Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Dev. Neuropsychol. 13, 397–445 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., Naoi, N., Kojima, S.: Neural attunement processes in infants during the acquisition of a language-specific phonemic contrast. J. Neurosci. 27, 315–321 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., Hebden, J.C., Dupoux, E.: Optical imaging of infants’ neurocognitive development: recent advances and perspectives. Dev. Neurobiol. 68, 712–728 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V.B.: The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain 120(Pt 4), 701–722 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A.: Innateness, autonomy, universality? Neurobiological approaches to language. Behav. Brain Sci. 19, 611–631 (1996)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A.: Genes, language disorders, and developmental archaeology: what role can neuroimaging play? In: Rice, M., Warren, S. (eds.) Developmental Language Disorders: From Phenotypes to Etiologies, pp. 291–328. Erlbaum, Mahwah (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A.: Language universals in the brain: how linguistic are they? In: Christiansen, M.H., Collins, C., Edelman, S. (eds.) Language Universals, pp. 224–252. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Basho, S.: Are nonlinguistic functions in “Broca’s area” prerequisites for language acquisition? FMRI findings from an ontogenetic viewpoint. Brain Lang. 89, 329–336 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Courchesne, E.: The duplicity of plasticity: a conceptual approach to the study of early lesion and developmental disorders. In: Ernst, M., Rumsey, J. (eds.) The Foundation and Future of Functional Neuroimaging in Child Psychiatry, pp. 335–365. Cambridge University Press, New York (2000)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Palmer, E.D.: Language and neurophysiological development. In: Rickheit, G., Strohner, H. (eds.) Communicative Competence. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Rothermel, R.D., Behen, M.E., Muzik, O., Mangner, T.J., Chakraborty, P.K., Chugani, H.T.: Brain organization of language after early unilateral lesion: a PET study. Brain Lang. 62, 422–451 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Rothermel, R.D., Behen, M.E., Muzik, O., Chakraborty, P.K., Chugani, H.T.: Language organization in patients with early and late left hemisphere lesion: a PET study. Neuropsychologia 37, 545–557 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R.-A., Kleinhans, N., Courchesne, E.: Broca’s area and the discrimination of frequency transitions: a functional MRI study. Brain Lang. 76, 70–76 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H.J., Nicol, J.L., Barss, A., Forster, K.I., Garrett, M.F.: Syntactically based sentence processing classes: evidence from event-related brain potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 151–165 (1991)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., Simon, H.A.: GPS, a program that simulates human thought. In: Feigenbaum, E., Feldman, J. (eds.) Computers and Thought, pp. 279–293. McGraw-Hill, New York (1963)

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, A.J., Pancheva, R., Ozawa, K., Neville, H.J., Ullman, M.T.: An event-related fMRI study of syntactic and semantic violations. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 30, 339–364 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newmeyer, F.J.: Linguistic Theory in America, 2nd edn. Academic Press, Orlando (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ni, W., Constable, R.T., Mencl, W.E., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K., Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Gore, J.C., Shankweiler, D.: An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. J. Cognitive Neurosci. 12, 120–133 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nishitani, N., Schurmann, M., Amunts, K., Hari, R.: Broca’s region: from action to language. Physiology (Bethesda) 20, 60–69 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nystrom, L.E., Delgado, M.R., Sabb, F.W., Noll, D.C., Choen, J.D.: Dynamics of fMRI: Broca’s area activation reflects independent effects of duration and intensity of working memory processes. Neuroimage 7, S7 (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Leary, D.D., Nakagawa, Y.: Patterning centers, regulatory genes and extrinsic mechanisms controlling arealization of the neocortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 14–25 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ojemann, G.A.: Cortical organization of language. J. Neurosci. 11, 2281–2287 (1991)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pantano, P., Baron, J.C., Samson, Y., Bousser, M.G., Desrouesne, C., Comar, D.: Crossed cerebellar diaschisis. Brain 109, 677–694 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pantev, C., Oostenveld, R., Engelien, A., Ross, B., Roberts, L.E., Hoke, M.: Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392, 811–814 (1998)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Passingham, R.E., Stephan, K.E., Kotter, R.: The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 606–616 (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, J.A.: Sensorimotor cortex as a critical component of an ’extended’ mirror neuron system: Does it solve the development, correspondence, and control problems in mirroring? Behav. Brain Funct. 4, 47 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R.A., Wagner, A.D., Prull, M.W., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., Gabrieli, J.D.: Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 10, 15–35 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K.R.: Conjectures and Refutations: The Growth of Scientific Knowledge. Routledge & Kegan Paul, Neuw York (1965)

    Google Scholar 

  • Puce, A., Perrett, D.: Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 358, 435–445 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redcay, E.: The superior temporal sulcus performs a common function for social and speech perception: implications for the emergence of autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 123–142 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ressel, V., Wilke, M., Lidzba, K., Lutzenberger, W., Krageloh-Mann, I.: Increases in language lateralization in normal children as observed using magnetoencephalography. Brain Lang. 106, 167–176 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rilling, J.K., Glasser, M.F., Preuss, T.M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., Behrens, T.E.: The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 426–428 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Craighero, L.: The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., Russell, T., Overmeyer, S., Brammer, M.J., Bullmore, E.T., Sharma, T., Simmons, A., Williams, S.C., Giampietro, V., Andrew, C.M., Taylor, E.: Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. Neuroimage 13, 250–261 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadato, N., Okada, T., Honda, M., Yonekura, Y.: Critical period for cross-modal plasticity in blind humans: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 16, 389–400 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmelin, R.: Clinical neurophysiology of language: the MEG approach. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 237–254 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santi, A., Grodzinsky, Y.: Working memory and syntax interact in Broca’s area. Neuroimage 37, 8–17 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saur, D., Kreher, B.W., Schnell, S., Kummerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.S., Umarova, R., Musso, M., Glauche, V., Abel, S., Huber, W., Rijntjes, M., Hennig, J., Weiller, C.: Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 105, 18035–18040 (2008)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F., Cours de Linguistique Générale. Payot, Paris (1915/1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlaggar, B.L., O’Leary, D.: Potential of visual cortex to develop an array of functional units unique to somatosensory cortex. Science 252, 1556–1560 (1991)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaggar, B.L., Brown, T.T., Lugar, H.M., Visscher, K.M., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, S.E.: Functional neuroanatomical differences between adults and school-age children in the processing of single words. Science 296, 1476–1479 (2002)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K., Winner, E.: Effects of music training on the child’s brain and cognitive development. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 219–230 (2005)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J.R.: Speech Acts. An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1969)

    Google Scholar 

  • Seghier, M.L., Lazeyras, F., Pegna, A.J., Annoni, J.M., Zimine, I., Mayer, E., Michel, C.M., Khateb, A.: Variability of fMRI activation during a phonological and semantic language task in healthy subjects. Hum. Brain Mapp. 23, 140–155 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T.: From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1988)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shtyrov, Y., Pulvermuller, F.: Early MEG activation dynamics in the left temporal and inferior frontal cortex reflect semantic context integration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1633–1642 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Signoret, J.L., Castaigne, P., Lhermitte, F., Abelanet, R., Lavorel, P.: Rediscovery of Leborgne’s brain: anatomical description with CT scan. Brain Lang. 22, 303–319 (1984)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, W.: Consciousness and the binding problem. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 929, 123–146 (2001)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F.: Verbal Behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York (1957)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Staudt, M., Lidzba, K., Grodd, W., Wildgruber, D., Erb, M., Krageloh-Mann, I.: Right-hemispheric organization of language following early left-sided brain lesions: functional MRI topography. Neuroimage 16, 954–967 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Rauch, S.: Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain Lang. 52, 452–473 (1996)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sur, M., Leamey, C.A.: Development and plasticity of cortical areas and networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 251–262 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sur, M., Pallas, S.L., Roe, A.W.: Cross-modal plasticity in cortical development: differentiation and specification of sensory neocortex. Trends Neurosci. 13, 227–233 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szaflarski, J.P., Schmithorst, V.J., Altaye, M., Byars, A.W., Ret, J., Plante, E., Holland, S.K.: A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language development in children 5 to 11 years old. Ann. Neurol. 59, 796–807 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P.M., Cannon, T.D., Narr, K.L., van Erp, T., Poutanen, V.P., Huttunen, M., Lonnqvist, J., Standertskjold-Nordenstam, C.G., Kaprio, J., Khaledy, M., Dail, R., Zoumalan, C.I., Toga, A.W.: Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1253–1258 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson-Schill, S.L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G.K., Farah, M.J.: Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 94, 14792–14797 (1997)

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Uchiyama, Y., Toyoda, H., Honda, M., Yoshida, H., Kochiyama, T., Ebe, K., Sadato, N.: Functional segregation of the inferior frontal gyrus for syntactic processes: a functional magnetic-resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Res. 61, 309–318 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheless, J.W., Castillo, E., Maggio, V., Kim, H.L., Breier, J.I., Simos, P.G., Papanicolaou, A.C.: Magnetoencephalography (MEG) and magnetic source imaging (MSI). Neurologist 10, 138–153 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xiong, J., Rao, S., Jerabek, P., Zamarripa, F., Woldorff, M., Lancaster, J., Fox, P.T.: Intersubject variability in cortical activations during a complex language task. Neuroimage 12, 326–339 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaehle, T., Wüstenberg, T., Meyer, M., Jäncke, L.: Evidence for rapid auditory perception as the foundation of speech processing: a sparse temporal sampling fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2447–2456 (2004)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this review was supported by the National Institutes of Health, grants R01-NS43999, R01-DC006155 and R01-MH081023. Thanks to Patricia Shih for help in editing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ralph-Axel Müller .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Müller, RA. (2013). Neurobiology: Language By, In, Through and Across the Brain. In: Binder, PM., Smith, K. (eds) The Language Phenomenon. The Frontiers Collection. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-36086-2_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-36086-2_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-36085-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-36086-2

  • eBook Packages: Physics and AstronomyPhysics and Astronomy (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics