Skip to main content

Abstract

Vocal communication plays a particularly important role in the regulation of social interactions and in the coordination of activities in many mammals and birds that are organised into social groups. Previous research on the function and evolution of vocal signals has mainly considered dyadic interactions of a signaller and its addressed receiver. However, in social groups it is likely that additional individuals attend to dyadic communication and that they use this information to their own benefit, sometimes at a cost to the signaller. To improve existing communication models, benefits and costs of vocal communication caused by bystanders must therefore also be considered. Here we discuss vocal communication in social groups and identify the effects of additional individuals on signalling interactions, concentrating on audience effects, eavesdropping and group coordination. First, a review of the existing literature reveals that the presence of an audience, i.e., additional individuals within the signalling range, clearly affects the outcome of communicative interactions, and that individuals modulate their signalling behaviour according to the presence of bystanders or a particular category of bystanders in a variety of contexts. Second, social knowledge acquired by eavesdropping on the communicative network within a group influences not only future actions, but can also provide individual benefits for eavesdroppers, whereas mutual eavesdropping can structure cooperation and alliance formation, and, hence, contribute to long-term group stability. Third, communicative networks also provide a means to facilitate the maintenance of group cohesion and decisionmaking processes. In conclusion, cost-benefit analyses at the level of dyadic interactions reveal clear differences with communication networks, where repeated interactions with multiple partners are considered. Future communication models and empirical studies should therefore consider the composition of the entire communication network as well as the effects of repeated interactions to fully understand signalling interactions in social groups.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alexander RD (1974) The evolution of social behavior. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 5:325-383

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauers KA (1993) A functional analysis of staccato grunt vocalizations in the stumptailed macaque (Macaca arctoides). Ethology 94:147-161

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell MBV (2007) Cooperative begging in banded mongoose pups. Curr Biol 17:717-721

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bell MBV (2008) Receiver identity modifies begging intensity independent of need in banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) pups. Behav Ecol 19:1087-1094

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2003) Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302:1234-1236

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal XE, Page RA, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2007) Cues for eavesdroppers: do frog calls indicate prey density and quality? Am Nat 169:409-415

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertram BCR (1978) Living in groups: predators and prey. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 64-96

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S (1991) The coordination of spatial position: a field study of the vocal behaviour of adult female squirrel monkeys. Anim Behav 41:89-102

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S (1993) Vocal coordination of troop movement among white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Am J Primatol 30:85-100

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S (2000) Social manipulation within and between troops mediates primate group movement. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 421-469

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S, Garber PA (2000) On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnie KE, Earley RL (2007) Expanding the scope for social information use. Anim Behav 74:171-181

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of Animal Communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland/MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar T, Kijne M, Kortschal K (2001) Food calling in ravens: are yells referential signals? Anim Behav 61:949-958

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RW (1981) Distance vocalizations of Guinea baboons (Papio papio): an analysis of function. Behaviour 78:283-312

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (2008) Birdsong: Biological Themes and Variations. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2005) Social complexity and the information acquired during eavesdropping by primates and other animals. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 583-603

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Silk JB (1995) The responses of female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) to anomalous social interactions: evidence for causal reasoning? J Comp Psychol 109:134-141

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Conradt L, Roper TJ (2003) Group decision-making in animals. Nature 421:155-158

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Conradt L, Roper TJ (2007) Democracy in animals: the evolution of shared group decisions. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:2317-2326

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Covas R, McGregor PK, Doutrelant C (2007) Cooperation and communication networks. Behav Proc 76:149-151

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockford C, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2007) Baboons eavesdrop to deduce mating opportunities. Anim Behav 73:885-890

    Google Scholar 

  • Dabelsteen T (2005) Public, private or anonymous? Facilitating and countering eavesdropping. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 38-62

    Google Scholar 

  • Danchin E, Giraldeau L-A, Valone TJ, Wagner RH (2004) Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487-491

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dechmann DKN, Heucke SL, Giuggioli L, Safi K, Voigt CC, Wikelski M (2009) Experimental evidence for group hunting via eavesdropping in echolocating bats. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2721-2728

    Google Scholar 

  • Deecke VB, Ford JKB, Slater PJB (2005) The vocal behaviour of mammal-eating killer whales: communicating with costly calls. Anim Behav 69:395-405

    Google Scholar 

  • DiBitetti MS (2003) Food-associated call of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour 140:565-592

    Google Scholar 

  • Elgar MA (1986) House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible. Anim Behav 34:169-174

    Google Scholar 

  • Engh AL, Hoffmeier RR, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2006) Who, me? Can baboons infer the target of vocalizations? Anim Behav 71:381-387

    Google Scholar 

  • Engh AL, Hoffmeier RR, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2009) O brother, where art thou? The varying influence of older siblings in rank acquisition by female baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:97-104

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans CS (1997) Referential signals. In: Owings DH, Beacher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives of Ethology. Plenum Press, New York, pp 99-143

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans CS, Evans L, Marler P (1993) On the meaning of alarm calls: functional reference in an avian vocal system. Anim Behav 46:23-38

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C (2004) Reciprocal recognition in sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) and redfronted lemur (Eulemur fulvus rufus) alarm calls. Anim Cogn 7:45-52

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C (in press a) Costs of alarm calling: lemur alarm calls attract fossas. Lemur News

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C (in press b) Predation on primates. In: Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit R, Silk J (eds) The Evolution of Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262-275

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens J (2001) On the vocal expression of emotion. A multi-parametric analysis of different states of aversion in the squirrel monkey. Behaviour 138:97-116

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Pyritz LW, Kappeler PM (in press) Coordination of group movements in non-human primates. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Ellwart S, Kappeler PM (eds) Coordination in Human and Non-human Primate Groups. Springer, Heidelberg

    Google Scholar 

  • Gautier-Hion A, Quris R, Gautier J-P (1983) Monospecific vs polyspecific life: a comparative study of foraging and antipredatory tactics in a community of Cercopithecus monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:325-335

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfray HCJ (1995) Signaling of need between parents and young: parentoffspring conflict and sibling rivalry. Am Nat 146:1-24

    Google Scholar 

  • Götz T, Verfuss UK, Schnitzler H-U (2006) ‘Eavesdropping’ in wild roughtoothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis)? Biol Lett 2:5-7

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69-73

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grinnell J, McComb K (2001) Roaring and social communication in African lions: the limitations imposed by listeners. Anim Behav 62:93-98

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros-Louis J (2004a) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Anim Behav 67:431-440

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros-Louis J (2004b) Responses of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) to naturalistic and experimentally presented food-associated calls. J Comp Psychol 118:396-402

    Google Scholar 

  • Gyger M, Karakashian SJ, Marler P (1986) Avian alarm calling: is there an audience effect? Anim Behav 34:1570-1572

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1996) The Evolution of Communication. Bradford/MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1998) Functional referents and acoustic similarity: field playback experiments with rhesus monkeys. Anim Behav 55:1647-1658

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Marler P (1993) Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behav Ecol 4:206-212

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich B, Marzluff JM (1991) Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28:13-21

    Google Scholar 

  • Hingee M, Magrath R (2009) Flights of fear: a mechanical wing whistle sounds the alarm in a flocking bird. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:4173-4179

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E (2002) Information: its interpretation, its inheritance, and its sharing. Philos Sci 69:578-605

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone RA (1998) Efficacy and honesty in communication between relatives. Am Nat 152:45-58

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone RA (2004) Begging and sibling competition: how should offspring respond to their rivals? Am Nat 163:388-406

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karakashian SJ, Gyger M, Marler P (1988) Audience effect on alarm calling in chickens (Gallus gallus). J Comp Psychol 102:129-135

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kerth G, Ebert C, Schmidtke C (2006) Group decision making in fission-fusion societies: evidence from two-field experiments in Bechstein’s bats. Proc R Soc Lond B 273:2785-2790

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchen DM (2004) Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim Behav 67:125-139

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda H, Shimooka Y, Suguira H (2008) Effects of caller activity and habitat visibility on contact call rate of wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Am J Primatol 70:1055-1063

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in Groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32:4-14

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lea AJ, Barrera JP, Tom LM, Blumstein DT (2008) Heterospecific eavesdropping in a nonsocial species. Behav Ecol 19:1041-1046

    Google Scholar 

  • Leighty KA, Soltis J, Wesolek CM, Savage A (2008) Rumble vocalizations mediate interpartner distance in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Anim Behav 76:1601-1608

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Roux A, Cherry MI, Manser MB (2008) The audience effect in a facultatively social mammal, the yellow mongoose, Cynictis penicillata. Anim Behav 75:943-949

    Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2007) A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other’s aerial alarm calls. Behav Ecol 18:944-951

    Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:2315-2324

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2009) The generation of functionally referential and motivational vocal signals in mammals. In: Brudzynsky SM (ed) Handbook of Mammalian Vocalization. Elsevier, New York, pp 479-488

    Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB, Bell MB, Fletcher LB (2001) The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:2485-2491

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1992) Functions of arousal and emotion in primate communication: a semiotic approach. In: Nishida T, McGrew WS, Marler P, Pickford M, de Waal FBM (eds) Topics in Primatology. Vol 1: Human Origins. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp 225-233

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler P, Dufty A, Pickert R (1986) Vocal communication in the domestic chicken. II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? Anim Behav 34:194-198

    Google Scholar 

  • Masataka N (1989) Motivational referents of contact calls in Japanese macaques. Ethology 80:265-273

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal Signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • McComb K, Reby D (2005) Vocal communication networks in large terrestrial mammals. In: McGregor P (ed) Animal communication networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 372-389

    Google Scholar 

  • McComb K, Moss C, Sayialel S, Baker L (2000) Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Anim Behav 59:1103-1109

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McComb K, Moss C, Durant SM, Baker L, Sayialel S (2001) Matriarchs as respositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science 292:491-494

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw WS, Bshary R (2002) Association of terrestrial mangabeys (Cercocebus atys) with arboreal monkeys: experimental evidence for the effects of reduced ground predator pressure on habitat use. Int J Primatol 23:311-325

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor PK (2005) Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor PK, Dabelsteen T (1996) Communication networks. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca/NY, pp 409-425

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehlman PT (1996) Branch shaking and related displays in wild Barbary macaques. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG (eds) Evolution and Ecology of Macaque Societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 503-526

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM (2004a) Do male black-capped chickadees eavesdrop on song contests? A multi-speaker playback experiment. Behaviour 141:125-139

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM (2004b) Overlapping and matching in song contests of black-capped chickadees. Anim Behav 67:441-450

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM, Boag PT (2002) Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. Science 296:873

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1977) On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am Nat 111:855-869

    Google Scholar 

  • Mougeot F, Bretagnolle V (2000) Predation as a cost of sexual communication in nocturnal seabirds: an experimental approach using acoustic signals. Anim Behav 60:647-656

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller CA, Manser MB (2008) The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms. Anim Behav 75:897-904

    Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M, Todt D (1997) Effects of dyadic vocal interactions on other conspecific receivers in nightingales. Anim Behav 54:1535-1543

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M, Fichtel C, Todt D (1999) Nightingales respond more strongly to vocal leaders of simulated dyadic interactions. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:537-542

    Google Scholar 

  • Otter K, Mcgregor PK, Terry AMR, Burford FRL, Peake TM, Dabelsteen T (1999) Do female great tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping? A field study using interactive song playback. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:1305-1309

    Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Leger DW (1980) Chatter vocalizations of California ground squirrels: predator-social-role specificity. Z Tierpsychol 54:163-184

    Google Scholar 

  • Papworth S, Böse A-S, Barker J, Schel AM, Zuberbühler K (2008) Male blue monkeys alarm call in response to danger experienced by others. Biol Lett 4:472-475

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paz-y-Miño G, Bond AB, Kamill AC, Balda RP (2004) Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature 430:778-781

    Google Scholar 

  • Peake TM (2005) Eavesdropping in communication networks. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 13-37

    Google Scholar 

  • Peake TM, Terry AMR, McGregor PK, Dablesteen T (2001) Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male-to-male vocal interactions. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1183-1187

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pereira ME, Macedonia JM (1991) Ringtailed lemur anti-predator calls denote predator class, not response urgency. Anim Behav 41:543-544

    Google Scholar 

  • Poole JH, Payne K, Langbauer WR Jr, Moss CJ (1988) The social contexts of some very low frequency calls of African elephants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:385-392

    Google Scholar 

  • Price K, Harvey H, Ydenberg R (1996) Begging tactics of nestling yellow-headed blackbirds, Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus, in relation to need. Anim Behav 51:421-435

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford AN (2004) Vocal coordination of group movement by green woodhoopoes (Phoeniculus purpureus). Ethology 110:11-20

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford AN, Ridley AR (2006) Recruitment calling: a novel form of extended parental care in an altricial species. Cur Biol 16:1700-1704

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM, Johnstone RA (2003) Spontaneous emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs. Nature 423:432-434

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reby D, Cargnelutti B, Hewison AJM (1999) Contexts and possible functions of barking in roe deer. Anim Behav 57:1121-1128

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rendall D, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2000) Proximate factors mediating ‘contact’ calls in adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) and their infants. J Comp Psychol 114:36-46

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley AR, Child MF, Bell MBV (2007) Interspecific audience effects on the alarm-calling behaviour of a kleptoparasitic bird. Biol Lett 3:589-591

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signalling Systems. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Semple S, Gerald MS, Suggs DN (2009) Bystanders affect the outcome of motherinfant interactions in rhesus macaques. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2257-2262

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1990) The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Anim Behav 40:754-764

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070-1094

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman PW (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science 197:1246-1253

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529-536

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB (2001) Grunts, girneys, and good intentions: the origins of strategic commitment in nonhuman primates. In: Nesse R (ed) Evolution and the Capacity for Commitment. Russell Sage Press, New York, pp 138-157

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB, Kaldor E, Boyd R (2000) Cheap talk when interests conflict. Anim Behav 59:423-432

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skyrms B (2009) Evolution of signalling systems with multiple senders and receivers. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 364:771-779

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff CN, Kiriazis J, Fischer C, Creef E (1991) Semantic information distinguishing individual predators in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Anim Behav 42:713-719

    Google Scholar 

  • Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K (2007) Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:17228-17233

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith WJ (1981) Referents of animal communication. Anim Behav 29:1273-1275

    Google Scholar 

  • Struhsaker TT (1967) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social Communication among Primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 281-384

    Google Scholar 

  • Sumpter DJT, Brännström A (2008) Synergy in social communication. In: d’Ettore P, Hughes DP (eds) Sociobiology of Communication: An Interdisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 191-208

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend SW, Deschner T, Zuberbühler K (2008) Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS ONE 3:e243, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002431

    Google Scholar 

  • Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2004) Coordination of group movements in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi). Behaviour 141:1103-1120

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik CP (1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87:120-144

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, St Clair JJH (2007) Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biol Lett 3:632-634

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wich SA, de Vries H (2006) Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 273:735-740

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich SA, Sterck EHM (2003) Possible audience effect in Thomas langurs (primates; Presbytis thomasi): an experimental study on male loud calls in response to a tiger model. Am J Primatol 60:155-159

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson GS, Boughman JW (1998) Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. Anim Behav 55:337-350

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolters S, Zuberbühler K (2003) Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell’s monkeys: the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour 140:371-385

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright J, Leonard ML (2002) The Evolution of Begging: Competition, Cooperation and Communication. Kluwer, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (1977) The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). J Theor Biol 67:603-605

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zemel A, Lubin Y (1995) Inter-group competition and stable group sizes. Anim Behav 50:485-488

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2008) Audience effects. Curr Biol 18:R189-R190

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1999) Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. J Comp Psychol 113:33-42

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuk M, Kolluru GR (1998) Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. Q Rev Biol 73:415-438

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fichtel, C., Manser, M. (2010). Vocal communication in social groups. In: Kappeler, P. (eds) Animal Behaviour: Evolution and Mechanisms. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-02624-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics