Skip to main content

Cultural Traits and Multidisciplinary Dialogue

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Understanding Cultural Traits

Abstract

Every discipline poses its research questions in specific ways and thus uses concepts that are suited to find answers in its well defined disciplinary frameworks. Accordingly, the idea of cultural trait has been used and developed in several disciplines, often without any reference to each other. The result has often been non-communicability across different fields. We first show, by means of two examples, that the lack of deep interdisciplinary dialogue and reciprocal understanding has generated some harsh controversies. We argue that our book Understanding Cultural Traits: A Multidisciplinary Perspective on Cultural Diversity tries to overcome not only the gap between cultural traits specialists and other scientists, but also the barriers among the researchers who are not specialists on cultural traits but that nevertheless directly or indirectly use cultural traits in their different disciplines.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Even in fields where “cultural traits” are not systematically cited, we may recognize the use of some analog notion. Comparative approaches in anthropology, history, and cultural studies, for example, make extensive use of the idea of a cultural trait. A famous example is Diamond (1999, 2005, 2012) and Diamond and Robinson (2010). Other, less historical, methodologically relevant texts are found in the ‘ecology of culture’, with authors such as Ingold (2000, 2011), or Sperber (1996, 2000) with his “epidemiology of culture” approach. Cross-cultural studies have specific methodology texts such as Hofstede (2001) and Minkov (2012). Beyond methodological and theoretical literature, we find many studies on the importance of cultural diversity and cross-culturalization for facing the challenges of our time, often promoted by United Nations and other political organisms. An example among many others can be Johnston et al. (2012).

  2. 2.

    Memetics is elaborated in books by Richard Dawkins, Daniel Dennett, Susan Blackmore, Richard Brodie, Robert Aunger, Tim Tyler. For a critical approach, see Distin (2004).

  3. 3.

    On evolutionary archaeology see Tëmkin (this volume), Carignani (this volume). Theoretical frameworks and findings are published in books such as O’Brien and Lyman (2000, 2003), O’Brien et al. (2005), and Shennan (2003, 2009). The relevance of phylogenetic thinking to linguistics is addressed in Da Milano and Puddu (this volume), and extensively exemplified below in Sect. 1.1. Carignani (this volume) provides examples of evolutionary thinking in engineering and technology studies.

  4. 4.

    Performing several tests and considering some potential objections and overlapping clines, Atkinson maintained distance from Africa as a significant predictor of phonemic diversity. Just like the much more marked pattern observed in neutral genetic markers, the gradient of phonemic diversity would be a ‘serial founder effect’, i.e., the signature of a series of population size reductions (bottlenecks) during the expansion of Homo sapiens out of Africa, dating 50–70,000 years ago according to the model used by Atkinson.

  5. 5.

    A whole special issue of Linguistic Topology (Bybee 2011) was published to discuss Atkinson’s method and results, and their implications. Cysouw et al. (2012), Van Tuyl and Pereltsvaig (2012), and Wang et al. (2012) responded on Science, then Atkinson (2012a) addressed their criticisms. Then there was one more exchange between Jaeger et al. (2012) and Atkinson (2012b). Afterwards, Hunley et al. (2012) published a “rejection” on the Proceedings of the Royal Society. Statistical methodologies, in particular arbitrary choices and errors, were discussed as the same data got re-analyzed making the trend disappear and yielding different and multiple origin points, but here we are more interested in discussions on phonemic data themselves.

  6. 6.

    This idea was expressed in the 1960s by Chistovich and Kozhevnikov in the “Leningrad school” of linguistics (Greenberg 2006). See also Faber (1990).

  7. 7.

    Just as cultural traits are the real building block of Mesoudi et al.’s proposal, they are also a major point of contention in the clash with Tim Ingold. See for example the following passage: “…Mesoudi et al.’s […] characterization is not merely anachronistic. It is also an affront to the millions of intelligent human beings for whom traditions are real and important but who are not, on that account, trait-bearing cultural clones whose only role in life is to express – in their behaviour, artefacts and organizations – information that has been transmitted to them from previous generations, only to have their performances observed and recorded in their natural habitat, along with other forms of wildlife, by intruding scientists. The genealogy of cultural traits that Mesoudi et al. propose, under the rubric of ‘comparative anthropology’, is a parody of history in which agency, power and social relations are but the ephemeral effects of proximate causes whose ultimate source is supposed to lie in capacities and dispositions bequeathed to individuals as an ancestral legacy, independently and in advance of their life in the world” (Ingold 2007: 14, emphasis added).

References

  • Atkinson, Q. D. (2011). Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science (New York, N.Y.), 332(6027), 346–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, Q. D. (2012a). Response to comments on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6069), 657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, Q. D. (2012b). Response to comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6072), 1042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blute, M. (2010). Darwinian sociocultural evolution: Solutions to dilemmas in cultural and social theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brambilla, R., & Serrelli, E. (2016). The goals and conditions of successful interdisciplinarity. Some critical guidelines in planning, managing and evaluating interdisciplinary projects. Paradigmi. Rivista di critica filosofica, forthcoming.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. (Ed.). (2011). The vanishing phonemes debate, apropos of Atkinson 2011. Linguistic Typology, 15(2), Special Issue, 147–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cysouw, M., Dediu, D., & Moran, S. (2012). Comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6069), 657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (1999). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (2005). Collapse: How societies choose to fail or succeed. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (2012). The world until yesterday: What can we learn from traditional societies? New York: Viking Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J., & Robinson, J. A. (Eds.). (2010). Natural experiments of history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Distin, K. (2004). The selfish meme: A critical reassessment. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Distin, K. (2010). Cultural evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Faber, A. (1990). Phonemic segmentation as epiphenomenon evidence from the history of alphabetic writing. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research 1990, SR-101/102, pp. 28–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, S. (2006). Production dynamics of speech. In P. Divenyi, S. Greenberg, G. Meyer (Eds.), Dynamics of speech production and perception (pp. 3–6). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS Press, NATO Science Series, I: Life and Behavioural Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heggarty, P. (2013). Ultraconserved words and Eurasiatic? The “faces in the fire” of language prehistory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(35), E3254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunley, K., Bowern, C., & Healy, M. (2012). Rejection of a serial founder effects model of genetic and linguistic coevolution. Proceedings Biological Sciences/the Royal Society, 279(1736), 2281–2288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment: Essays in livelihood, dwelling, and skill. London/New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2007). The trouble with “evolutionary biology”. Anthropology Today, 23(2), 13–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2011). Being alive. Essays on movement, knowledge and description. Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. F., Pontillo, D., & Graff, P. (2012). Comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6072), 1042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, B. R., et al. (2012). Water, cultural diversity, and global environmental change: Emerging trends, sustainable futures? Dordrecht/New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., Holden, C. J., & Shennan, S. (Eds.). (2005). The evolution of cultural diversity: A phylogenetic approach. Walnut Creek: Left Coast Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago/London: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 329–347. discussion 347–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2007). Science, evolution and cultural anthropology. A response to Ingold (this issue). Anthropology Today, 23(2), 18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minkov, M. (2012). Cross-cultural analysis: The science and art of comparing the world’s modern societies and their cultures. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2000). Applying evolutionary archaeology: A systematic approach. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2003). Cladistics and archaeology. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., & Schiffer, M. B. (2005). Archaeology as a process: Processualism and its progeny. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution (Monographs in population biology, Vol. 37). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagel, M., et al. (2013). Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(21), 8471–8476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. (2003). Genes, memes and human history: Darwinian archaeology and cultural evolution. London: Thames & Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. (Ed.). (2009). Pattern and process in cultural evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (Ed.). (2000). Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford: Oxfor University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tuyl, R., & Pereltsvaig, A. (2012). Comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6069), 657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, C.-C., et al. (2012). Comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science, 335(6069), 657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Our first thank goes to Luca Stanca, Director, CISEPS Center for Interdisciplinary Studies in Economics, Psychology, and Social Sciences at the University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy, whose support was so precious during these years. Thanks to his true engagement in interdisciplinarity, we had the necessary support to go along with this project. We also want to thank CISEPS for financial support, along with “Riccardo Massa” Department of Human Sciences at the same university.

Then, we would like to thank Luigino Bruni and Telmo Pievani, who had, more that 5 years ago, the first intuition of letting people from different disciplines meet in the University of Milano - Bicocca, sharing the interest in evolutionary studies. We took up their heritage by promoting a new wave of seminars, more focused on the concept of cultural traits, from which this book was born.

It was possible to realize this book thanks to all participants to the seminars we held. We thus want to thank all presenters and, especially, those who then agreed to become authors of this book. We want to highlight the fact that this is really a collective work, in which authors did not simply agree to write chapters assigned by the editors; on the contrary, almost all authors participated in the book creation process, constituting a research group, reviewing each others’ work, and stimulating interdisciplinary debate.

We also took advantage of cooperation with AppEEL – Applied Evolutionary Epistemology Lab, University of Lisbon, in particular with its greatly interdisciplinary research and dissemination project “Implementing the Extended Synthesis in Evolutionary Biology into the Sociocultural Domain” funded by the John Templeton Foundation. Fabrizio Panebianco kindly aknowledges the financial support of the ERC project TECTACOM – 324004. Emanuele Serrelli kindly acknowledges support from the John Templeton Foundation in the framework of the 2012/2013 project “Implementing the Extended Synthesis in Evolutionary Biology into the Sociocultural Domain” carried out at the Lisbon Applied Evolutionary Epistemology Lab (grant ID 36288).

At last, if this book appears as it is, this is greatly due to Springer’s referees and staff, and to the precious external referees from the many disciplines involved, who donated their time to ensure the scientific correctness of this book. On top of the overall referee process, they acted as an additional stimulus during the book construction process.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emanuele Serrelli .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Panebianco, F., Serrelli, E. (2016). Cultural Traits and Multidisciplinary Dialogue. In: Panebianco, F., Serrelli, E. (eds) Understanding Cultural Traits. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24349-8_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24349-8_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-24347-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-24349-8

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics