Skip to main content

Concept Composition in Frames: Focusing on Genitive Constructions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Frames and Concept Types

Part of the book series: Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy ((SLAP,volume 94))

Abstract

In this paper, we show how frames can be employed in the analysis of genitive constructions. We model the main approaches in the discussion about genitive constructions, i.e. the argument-only approach, the modifier-only approach and the split approach. Of these three, the split approach is modeled most naturally in frames. Thus, if frames are considered a cognitively adequate representation of concepts, our analysis supports the split approach to the interpretation of genitive constructions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The former construction ‘brother of John’ is often referred to as a ‘postnominal genitive of phrase’ (Barker to appear). Whether the latter construction ‘a brother of John’s’ is a genitive construction is controversial. While some, including Partee and Borschev (2003), classify ‘of John’s’ as a postnominal Saxon genitive, Barker (2004) and others argue that it is not a true genitive but a partitive construction.

  2. 2.

    Throughout this paper, we will concentrate on examples in which the main NP consists of a single noun. More complex NPs which involve relational adjectives, e.g., ‘John’s favorite movie’, will not be considered in detail here.

  3. 3.

    A deeper discussion of the relational status of nouns follows in Sect. 11.2.2. A comprehensive discussion is given in Löbner (2011).

  4. 4.

    Exceptions occur when the node’s referent is uniquely determined, as will be discussed in Sect. 11.2.2.

  5. 5.

    Note that this expression is not unique. We do not regard the dual question of which fragment of the λ-calculus is expressable by frames.

  6. 6.

    He calls them conceptual types but to avoid confusion with the type hierarchy and with logical types, we stick to ‘classes’ in this paper.

  7. 7.

    Again, please note that our examples are highly simplified.

  8. 8.

    Remember that Löbner calls ‘type’ what we call ‘class’.

  9. 9.

    We write predicates in the same fonts as in frames, i.e. A for an attribute and T for a type.

  10. 10.

    This is an artefact of the λ-notation. In the graph notation, the central node is closed iff it is determined by an incoming arc from another open node or from context. If a construction fills that node or destroys the connection to the open node, the central node is open. In λ-notation, there is no such straightforward constraint.

  11. 11.

    We write Pred (FC) to denote the predicative reading of the FC.

  12. 12.

    For example, it can be argued that the genitive construction should be made with a minimal frame for the concept, like the one in Fig. 11.18. This, in turn, opens the question about definition and existence of minimal frames.

References

  • Barker, Chris. 2004. Possessive weak definites. In Possessives and beyond: Semantics and syntax, ed. Y. Lander, J. Kim, and B.H. Partee, 89–113. Amherst: GLSA, Linguistic Department, University of Massachusets at Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, Chris. to appear. Possessives and relational nouns.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1992. Frames, concepts, and conceptual fields. In Frames, fields, and contrasts, ed. Adrienne Lehrer and Eva Feder Kittay, 21–74. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, Bob. 1992. The logic of typed feature structures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, K., and M. Zimmermann. 2003. Syntactic and semantic adnominal genitive. In A-symmetrien – A-symmetries, ed. C. Maienborn, 171–202. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, Per Anker, and Carl Vikner. 1994. Lexical knowledge and the semantic analysis of Danish genitive constructions. In Topics in knowledge-based NLP systems, ed. S.L. Hansen and H. Wegener, 37–55. Frederiksberg: Samfundslitteratur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, Per Anker, and Carl Vikner. 2004. The English prenominal genitive and lexical semantics. In Possessives and beyond: Semantics and syntax, ed. Yury A. Lander, Ji-Yung Kim, and Barbara H. Partee, 3–27. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Löbner, Sebastian. 1985. Definites. Journal of Semantics 4: 279–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Löbner, Sebastian. 2011. Concept types and determination. Journal of Semantics. Access published in May 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Löbner, Sebastian. 2013. Evidence for frames from human language. In Frames and concept types: Applications in language and philosophy, ed. Thomas Gamerschlag, Doris Gerland, Rainer Osswald, and Wiebke Petersen. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, Albert. 2013. Definite article asymmetries and concept types: Semantic and pragmatic uniqueness. In Frames and concept types: Applications in language and philosophy, ed. Thomas Gamerschlag, Doris Gerland, Rainer Osswald, and Wiebke Petersen. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, Barbara H. 1983/1997. Uniformity versus versatility: The genitive, a case study. In The handbook of logic and language, ed. Johan van Benthem and Alice ter Meulen, 464–470. Amsterdam/New York: Elsevier. Chapter Appendix to Theo Janssen (1997) Compositionality.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, Barbara H. 1986. Noun phrase interpretation and type-shifting principles. In Studies in discourse representation theory and the theory of generalized quantifiers, ed. D. de Jongh, J. Groenendijk, and M. Stokhof, chap. 15, 115–143. Dordrecht: Foris.

  • Partee, Barbara H., and Vladimir Borschev. 1998. Integrating lexical and formal semantics: Genitives, relational nouns, and type-shifting. In Proceedings of the second Tbilisi symposium on language, logic and computation, ed. R. Cooper and Th. Gamkrelidze, 229–241. Tbilisi: Center on Language, Logic, Speech: Tbilisi State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, Barbara H., and Vladimir Borschev. 2003. Genitives, relational nouns, and argument-modifier ambiguity. In Modifying adjuncts, ed. C. Maienborn, E. Lang, and C. Fabrizius-Hansen. Interface explorations, 67–112. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, Wiebke, and Tanja Osswald. 2012. A formal interpretation of concept types and type shifts. In Cognitive processes in language, ed. Krzysztof Kosecki and Janusz Badio. Volume 25 of Lodz studies in language, 183–191. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, Wiebke, and Markus Werning. 2007. Conceptual fingerprints: Lexical decomposition by means of frames – a neuro-cognitive model. In ICCS 2007, Sheffield, ed. U. Priss, S. Polovina, and R. Hill. Lecture notes in artificial intelligence, 415–428. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pustejovsky, James. 1995. The generative lexicon. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Søgaard, Anders. 2006. The semantics of possession in natural language and knowledge representation. Journal of Universal Language 6: 85–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vikner, C., and P.A. Jensen. 2002. A semantic analysis of the English genitive. Interaction of lexical and formal semantics. Studia Linguistica 56(2): 191–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Work on this paper was funded by the DFG, FOR 600. We would like to thank the audiences of ctf09 and Riga2010, an anonymous reviewer of an earlier version of this paper, and Marie-Luise Fischer, Janine Reinert, Eva Nowack, Albert Ortmann, Ralf Naumann and Sebastian Löbner for helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wiebke Petersen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Petersen, W., Osswald, T. (2014). Concept Composition in Frames: Focusing on Genitive Constructions. In: Gamerschlag, T., Gerland, D., Osswald, R., Petersen, W. (eds) Frames and Concept Types. Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy, vol 94. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01541-5_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics