Skip to main content

Development of Speech Perception

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Human Auditory Development

Part of the book series: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research ((SHAR,volume 42))

Abstract

During infancy, interactions with caretakers provide young humans with opportunities to discover important aspects of their world, including ways in which they and others communicate. In the current view, “communication” involves dynamic, real-time processing of multiple sources of information arising both within and between participants, directing attention, enabling co-action, and enriching understanding of the surrounding world (Hollich et al. 2000; Locke 2001).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N. (2007). What’s in a look? Developmental Science, 10, 48–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N., & Fiser, J. (2005). Methodological challenges for understanding cognitive development in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 92–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N., & Mehler, J. (2005). Near-infrared spectroscopy for functional studies of brain activity in human infants: Promise, prospects, and challenges. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 10, 11009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, B. A., & Newman, R. S. (2004). Listen to your mother! The role of talker familiarity in infant streaming. Cognition, 94, B45–B53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Early word-object associations and later language development. First Language, 27, 315–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 345–360.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D., Gooding, T. N., Orlando, C. B., & Quann, C. A. (2009). Development of phonological constancy: Toddlers’ perception of native- and Jamaican-accented words. Psychological Science, 20, 539–542.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R., & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16, 298–304.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2, 29–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46, 217–243.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boysson-Bardies, B. de, Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1984). Discernable differences in the babbling of infants according to target language. Journal of Child Language, 11, 1–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brent, M. R., & Cartwight, T. A. (1996). Distributional regularity and phonotactics are useful for segmentation. Cognition, 61, 93–125.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Bilingual and monolingual infant phonetic development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 455–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J., Floccia, C., Goslin, J., & Panneton, R. (2010). Infants’ discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar accents in speech. Infancy, 16, 392–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., Allen, J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Learning to segment speech using multiple cues: A connectionist model. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 221–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christophe, A., & Morton, J. (1998). Is Dutch native English? Linguistic analysis by 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 215–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christophe, A., Gout, A., Peperkamp, S., & Morgan, J. (2003). Discovering words in the continuous speech stream: The role of prosody. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 585–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. B., Diehl, R. L., Oakes, L. M., & Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Infant perception of /aba/ versus /apa/: Building a quantitative model of infant categorical discrimination. Developmental Psychology, 28, 261–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology 52, 337–367.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior & Development, 9, 133–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Montavont, A., Jobert, A., Allirol, L., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier, L., & Dehaene, S. (2010). Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks. Brain and Language, 114, 53–65.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, D. E., Emrich, H. M., Waller, C., Wieringac, B. M., Johannes, S., & Münted, T. F. (2000). Emotion/cognition-coupling in word recognition memory of depressive patients: An event-related potential study. Psychiatry Research, 96, 15–29.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1980). Discrimination of information for manner of articulation. Infant Behavior & Development, 3, 367–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Vigorrito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fais, L., Kajikawa, S., Amano, S., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Infant discrimination of a morphologically relevant word-final contrast. Infancy, 14, 488–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, N. H., Griffiths, T. L., & Morgan, J. L. (2009). The influence of categories on perception: Explaining the perceptual magnet effect as optimal statistical inference. Psychological Review, 116, 752–782.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46, 245–264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behaviour and Development, 8, 181–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27, 209–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mother’s and father’s speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477–501.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72, 1003–1015.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V.A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. Sekerina, E. M. Fernandez, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97–135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C., & Tokura, H. (1996). Acoustic cues to grammatical structure in infant-directed speech: Cross-linguistic evidence. Child Development, 67, 3192–3218.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., & Oberecker, R. (2008). The development of syntactic brain correlates during the first years of life. In A. D. Friederici & G. Thierry (Eds.), Trends in language acquisition research (pp. 215–231). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerken, L. A. (1996). Phonological and distributional information in syntax acquisition. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 411–425). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerken, L. A., Wilson, R., & Lewis, W. (2005). Infants can use distributional cues to form syntactic categories. Journal of Child Language, 32, 249–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19, 515–522.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Alioto, A. (1995). Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: Implications for language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 22, 703–726.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Psychological Science, 18, 254–260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). Developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65, 852–858.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hanulíková, A., McQueen, J. M., & Mitterer, H. (2010). Possible words and fixed stress in the segmentation of Slovak speech. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 555–579.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, P. (2000). Acquiring the phonology of lexical tone in infancy. Lingua, 110, 581–616.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, A., Tamekawa, Y., & Kiritani, S. (2001). Developmental change in auditory preference for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 44, 1189–1200.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, 3 (Serial No. 262).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollich, G., Newman, R. S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2005). Infants’ use of synchronized visual information to separate streams of speech. Child Development, 76, 598–613.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoonhorst, I., Colin, C., Markessis, E., Radeau, M., Deltenre, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2009). French native speakers in the making: From language-general to language-specific voicing boundaries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 353–366.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Houston, D. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1570–1582.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Houston, D. M., Horn, D. L., Qi, R., Ting, J., & Gao, S. (2007). Assessing speech discrimination in individual infants. Infancy, 12, 119–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houston-Price, C., & Nakai, S. (2004). Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures. Infant and Child Development, 13, 341–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. K., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 548–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. K., & Zamuner, T. S. (2010). Using infant and toddler testing methods in language acquisition research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental methods in language acquisition research (pp. 73–94). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Junge, C., Hagoort, P., Kooijman, V., & Cutler, A. (2010). Brain potentials for word segmentation at seven months predict later language development. In K. Franich, K. M. Iserman & L. L. Keil (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 209–220). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1–23.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, G. S., Cohn, J. F., & Moore, C. A. (1996). A combination of vocal f0 dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 67, 205–217.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (1998). The infant’s response to vocal affect in maternal speech. In C. Rovee-Collier (Ed.), Advances in Infancy Research, 12, 221–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother’s speech: Adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4, 85–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, C., & Lam, C. (2009). Age-specific preferences for infant-directed affective intent. Infancy, 14, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klee, T., Pearce, K., & Carson, D. (2000). Improving the positive predictive value of screening for developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 821–833.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kooijman, V., Hagoort, P., & Cutler, A. (2005). Electrophysiological evidence for prelinguistic infants’ word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 109–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, L.V., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277, 684–686.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 100, 9096–9101.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lasky, R. E., Syrdal-Lasky, A., & Klein, R. E. (1975). VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 215–225.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, H. M., Kuhl, P. K., & Tsao, F. M. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science 6F1–F10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., & Elwell, C. E. (2010). Illuminating the developing brain: The past, present and future of functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 34, 269–284.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (2001). First communion: The emergence of vocal relationships. Social Development, 10, 294–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11, F9–F16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10, 241–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattock, K., Molnar, M., Polka, L., & Burnham, D. (2008). The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life. Cognition, 106, 1367–1381.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattys, S. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Do infants segment words or recurring contiguous patterns? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 644–655.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mattys, S., White, L., & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: A hierarchical framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 477–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–B111.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCardle, P., Colombo, J., & Freund, L. (2009). Measuring language in infancy. In Colombo, J., McCardle, P., & Freund, L. (Eds.), Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and research disorders (pp. 1–12). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Anticipatory eye movements reveal infants’ auditory and visual categories. Infancy, 6, 203–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Infants are sensitive to within-category variation in speech perception. Cognition, 95, B15–B26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., Gervain, J., Endress, A. D., & Shukla, M. (2008). Mechanisms of language acquisition: Imaging and behavioral evidence. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 325–336). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–178.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moon, C., Panneton-Cooper, R., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior & Development, 16, 495–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D., Spence, M., & Katz, G. (1997). Six-month-olds’ categorization of natural infant directed utterances. Developmental Psychology, 33, 980–989.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. L., & Saffran, J. R. (1995). Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. Child Development, 66, 911–936.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Munhall, K. G., Jones, J. A., Callan, D. E., Kuratate, T., & Vatikiotis-Bateson, E. (2004). Visual prosody and speech intelligibility. Psychological Science, 15, 133–137.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Naigles, L. R. (2002). Form is easy, meaning is hard: Resolving a paradox in early child language. Cognition, 86, 157–199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 756–766.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W., & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., Paterson, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Early word segmentation by infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Infancy, 4, 251–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., Dilley, L. C., Jusczyk, A. M., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2005). English-learning infants’ segmentation of verbs from fluent speech. Language and Speech, 48, 279–298.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S. (2005). The cocktail party effect in infants revisited: Listening to one’s name in noise. Developmental Psychology, 41, 352–362.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S. (2006). Perceptual restoration in toddlers. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 625–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S. (2009). Infant’s listening in multitalker environments: Effect of the number of background talkers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 822–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S., & Hussain, I. (2006). Changes in infant preference for infant-directed speech in low and moderate noise by 4.5- to 13-month-olds. Infancy, 10, 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1996). The cocktail party effect in infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 1145–1156.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. S., Bernstein Ratner, N., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Dow, K. A. (2006). Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: A retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology, 42, 643–655.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nittrouer, S. (1996). Discriminability and perceptual weighting of some acoustic cues to speech perception by three-year-olds. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39, 278–297.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nittrouer, S. & Lowenstein, J. H. (2010). Learning to perceptually organize speech signals in native fashion. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127, 1624–1635.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nozza, R. J., Rossman, R. N. F., Bond, L. C., & Miller, S. L. (1990). Infant speech-sound discrimination in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87, 339–350.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nozza, R. J., Rossman, R. N. F., & Bond, L. C. (1991). Infant-adult differences in unmasked thresholds for the discrimination of consonant-vowel syllable pairs. Audiology, 30, 102–112.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nygaard, L. C., & Lunders, E. R. (2002). Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional tone of voice. Memory & Cognition, 30, 583–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panneton, R., Kitamura, C., Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Slow speech enhances younger, but not older infants’ perception of vocal emotion. Research in Human Development, Special Issue: The ecology of emotion in parenting relationships: Perspectives across the lifespan, 3, 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panneton-Cooper, R. (1993). The effect of prosody on young infants’ speech perception. In C. Rovee-Collier & L.P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in Infancy Research (pp. 137–167). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panneton-Cooper, R., & Aslin, R.N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first months after birth. Child Development, 61, 1584–1595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panneton-Cooper, R., & Aslin, R. N. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65, 1663–1677.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panneton-Cooper, R., Abraham, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 477–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pegg, J., Werker, J. F., & McLeod, P. J. (1992). Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 325–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovačić D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 100, 11702–11705.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, K. (1945). The intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polka, L., & Bohn, O-S. (2011). Natural Referent Vowel (NRV) framework: An emerging view of early phonetic development. Journal of Phonetics, doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.007.

  • Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of non-native vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421–435.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Polka, L., Rvachew, S., & Molnar, M. (2008). Speech perception by 6- to 8-month-olds in the presence of distracting sounds. Infancy, 13, 421–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 28, 349–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, C. W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2004). Auditory dominance and its change in the course of development. Child Development, 75, 1387–1401.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rost, G. C., & McMurray, B. (2010). Finding the signal by adding noise: The role of noncontrastive phonetic variability in early word learning. Infancy, 15, 680–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Werker, J. F., & Werner, L. A. (2006). The infant’s auditory world: Hearing, speech, and the beginnings of language. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (pp. 58108). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Santesso, D. L., Schmidt, L. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. Brain and Cognition, 65 14–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sato, Y., Sogabe, Y., & Mazuka, R. (2010). Discrimination of phonemic vowel length by Japanese infants. Developmental Psychology, 46, 106–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1995). Expression of emotion in voice and music. Journal of Voice, 9, 235–248.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmale, R., & Seidl, A. (2009). Accommodating variability in voice and foreign accent: Flexibility of early word representations. Developmental Science, 12, 583–601.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, L. S., Pascalis, O., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). A domain-general theory of the development of perceptual discrimination. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 197–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Developmental shifts in the discrimination of vowel contrasts in bilingual infants: Is the distributional account all there is to it? Developmental Science, 12, 874–887.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seidl, A., & Johnson, E. K. (2006). Infant word segmentation revisited: Edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science, 9, 565–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & Best, C. T. (2002). Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3, 365–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2008). Infant-directed speech is modulated by infant feedback. Infancy, 13, 410–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, M., Seidl, A., Kemler Nelson, D. G., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2003). The prosodic bootstrapping of phrases: Evidence from prelinguistic infants. Journal of Memory & Language, 49, 249–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, M., Conwell, E., Feldman, N., & Morgan, J. L. (2009). Statistical learning in language acquisition: Beyond demonstrations, towards a theory. Developmental Science, 12, 409–411.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381–382.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundara, M., & Scutellaro, A. (2010). Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. Journal of Phonetics, doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.006.

  • Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108, 232–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D. (2005). 11-month-olds’ knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental Science, 8, 432–443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73–108.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teinonen, T., Aslin, R. N., Alku, P., & Csibra, G. (2008). Visual speech contributes to phonetic learning in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 108, 850–855.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen, E. D., & Saffran, J. R. (2003). When cues collide: Use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 39, 706–716.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7, 53–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, K., & Fernald, A. (2006). Knowing what a novel word is not: Two-year-olds ‘listen through’ ambiguous adjectives in fluent speech. Cognition, 100, 389–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J., & Desjardins, R. N. (2002). Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 335–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardines, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11, 188–195.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 97, 466–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E., Bull, D., & Schneider, B. A. (1981). Infants’ detection of speech in noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 202–206.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2006). Perception of native and non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: Cross-language tests on adults and infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2285–2294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallabha, G. K., McClelland, J. L., Pons, F., Werker, J. F., & Amano, S. (2007). Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-directed speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 13273–13278.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van de Weijer, J. (1998). Language input for word discovery. Wageningen, the Netherlands: Ponsen & Looijen, BV.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M., Nakai, S., DePaolis, R. A., & Halle, P. (2004). The role of accentual pattern in early lexical representation. Journal of Memory & Language, 50, 336–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10, 159–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R. M. (1970). Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science, 167, 392–393.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316, 1159.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning & Development, 1, 197–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 7, 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., Polka, L., & Pegg, J. E. (1997). The conditioned head turn procedure as a method for assessing infant speech perception. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 171–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V. L., Casasola, M., & Stager, C. L. (1998). Acquisition of word-object associations by14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1289–1309.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., & Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103, 147–162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, L., Melborn, J. F., & Mattys, S. (2010). Segmentation by lexical subtraction in Hungarian speakers of second-language English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 544–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zangl, R., Klarman, L., Thal, D. J., Fernald, A., & Bates, E. (2005). Dynamics of word comprehension in infancy: Developments in timing, accuracy, and resistance to acoustic degradation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6, 179–208.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robin Panneton .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Panneton, R., Newman, R. (2012). Development of Speech Perception. In: Werner, L., Fay, R., Popper, A. (eds) Human Auditory Development. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, vol 42. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-1421-6_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics