Skip to main content

Modeling Decision-Making Systems in Addiction

  • Chapter
Computational Neuroscience of Drug Addiction

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Computational Neuroscience ((NEUROSCI,volume 10))

Abstract

This chapter describes addiction as a failure of decision-making systems. Existing computational theories of addiction have been based on temporal difference (TD) learning as a quantitative model for decision-making. In these theories, drugs of abuse create a non-compensable TD reward prediction error signal that causes pathological overvaluation of drug-seeking choices. However, the TD model is too simple to account for all aspects of decision-making. For example, TD requires a state-space over which to learn. The process of acquiring a state-space, which involves both situation classification and learning causal relationships between states, presents another set of vulnerabilities to addiction. For example, problem gambling may be partly caused by a misclassification of the situations that lead to wins and losses. Extending TD to include state-space learning also permits quantitative descriptions of how changing representations impacts patterns of intertemporal choice behavior, potentially reducing impulsive choices just by changing cause-effect beliefs. This approach suggests that addicts can learn healthy representations to recover from addiction. All the computational models of addiction published so far are based on learning models that do not attempt to look ahead into the future to calculate optimal decisions. A deeper understanding of how decision-making breaks down in addiction will certainly require addressing the interaction of drugs with model-based look-ahead decision mechanisms, a topic that remains unexplored.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    There are multiple decision factors often referred to as “impulsivity”, including the inability to inhibit a pre-potent response, the inability to inhibit an over-learned response, and an over-emphasis on immediate versus delayed rewards (which we are referring to here). These multiple factors seem to be independent (Reynolds et al. 2006) and to depend on different brain structures (Isoda and Hikosaka 2008) and we will not discuss the other factors here.

References

  • Ainslie G (1974) Impulse control in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav 21:485

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie G (2001) Breakdown of will. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen S, Harrison GW, Lau MI, Rutström EE (2008) Eliciting risk and time preferences. Econometrica 76:583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aragona BJ, Cleaveland NA, Stuber GD, Day JJ, Carelli RM, Wightman RM (2008) Preferential enhancement of dopamine transmission within the nucleus accumbens shell by cocaine is attributable to a direct increase in phasic dopamine release events. J Neurosci 28:8821

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Arbisi PA, Billington CJ, Levine AS (1999) The effect of naltrexone on taste detection and recognition threshold. Appetite 32:241

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Balleine BW (2001) Incentive processes in instrumental conditioning. In: Handbook of contemporary Learning Theories, p 307

    Google Scholar 

  • Balleine BW (2004) Incentive behavior. In: The behavior of the laboratory rat: a handbook with tests, p 436

    Google Scholar 

  • Balleine BW, Dickinson A (1998) Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology 37:407

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Balleine BW, Daw ND, O’Doherty JP (2008) Multiple forms of value learning and the function of dopamine. In: Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain, p 367

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes CA (1979) Memory deficits associated with senscence: A neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 93:74

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barto AG (1994) Adaptive critics and the basal ganglia. In: Models of information processing in the basal ganglia, p 215

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum W, Rachlin H (1969) Choice as time allocation. J Exp Anal Behav 12:861

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer HM, Glimcher P (2005) Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron 47:129

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Becker GS, Murphy KM (1988) A theory of rational addiction. J Polit Econ 96:675

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker GS, Grossman M, Murphy KM (1994) An empirical analysis of cigarette addiction. Am Econ Rev 84:396

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernheim BD, Rangel A (2004) Addiction and cue-triggered decision processes. Am Econ Rev 94:1558

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge KC (2007) The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191:391

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge CW, Arnsten AF, Foote SL (1993) Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive function: clinical implications of anatomical, electrophysiological and behavioural studies in animal models. Psychol Med 23:557

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ (1999) Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 146:447

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton ME (2002) Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry 52:976

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D (1989) Slow reacquisition following extinction: context, encoding, and retrieval mechanisms. J Exp Psychol, Anim Behav Processes 15:43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, Maren S (2006) Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry 60:352

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breland K, Breland M (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. Am Psychol 16:682

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burks SV, Carpenter JP, Goette L, Rustichini A (2009) Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:7745

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Childress AR, Ehrman R, Rohsenow DJ, Robbins SJ, O’Brien CP (1992) Classically conditioned factors in drug dependence. In: Substance abuse: a comprehensive textbook, p 56

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen CJ, Silberberg A, Hursh SR, Roma PG, Riley AL (2008) Demand for cocaine and food over time. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91:209

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chung SH, Herrnstein RJ (1967) Choice and delay of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 10:67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Corbit LH, Balleine BW (2000) The role of the hippocampus in instrumental conditioning. J Neurosci 20:4233

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cote D, Caron A, Aubert J, Desrochers V, Ladouceur R (2003) Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. J Gambl Stud 19:433

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courville AC (2006) A latent cause theory of classical conditioning. Doctoral dissertation, Carnegie Mellon University

    Google Scholar 

  • Custer RL (1984) Profile of the pathological gambler. J Clin Psychiatry 45:35

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Daw ND (2003) Reinforcement learning models of the dopamine system and their behavioral implications. Doctoral dissertation, Carnegie Mellon University

    Google Scholar 

  • Daw ND, Doya K (2006) The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:199

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Daw ND, Kakade S, Dayan P (2002) Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Netw 15:603

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P (2005) Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nat Neurosci 8:1704

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Daw ND, Courville AC, Touretzky DS (2006) Representation and timing in theories of the dopamine system. Neural Comput 18:1637

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dayan P (2002) Motivated reinforcement learning. Advances in neural information processing systems: proceedings of the 2002 conference

    Google Scholar 

  • Dayan P, Balleine BW (2002) Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36:285

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dayan P, Seymour B (2008) Values and actions in aversion. In: Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain, p 175

    Google Scholar 

  • Dayan P, Niv Y, Seymour B, Daw ND (2006) The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Netw 19:1153

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dezfouli A, Piray P, Keramati MM, Ekhtiari H, Lucas C, Mokri A (2009) A neurocomputational model for cocaine addiction. Neural Comput 21:2869

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • di Chiara G (1999) Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent associative learning disorder. Eur J Pharmacol 375:13

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson M, O’Connor J (2006) Gambling as an addictive behavior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Domjan M (1998) The principles of learning and behavior. Brooks/Cole

    Google Scholar 

  • Doya K (2000) Metalearning, neuromodulation, and emotion. In: Affective minds, p 101

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J (1999) Gambling and addiction. In: Getting hooked: rationality and addiction, p 208

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes WK (1943) Discriminative conditioning. I. A discriminative property of conditioned anticipation. J Exp Psychol 32:150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorillo CD, Newsome WT, Schultz W (2008) The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons. Nat Neurosci 11:966

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flagel SB, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (2008) Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to a reward-related cue: Influence on cocaine sensitization. Behav Brain Res 186:48

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T (2002) Time Discounting and time preference: A critical review. J Econ Lit 40:351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhs MC, Touretzky DS (2007) Context learning in the rodent hippocampus. Neural Comput 19:3172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gershman SJ, Blei DM, Niv Y (2010) Context, learning, and extinction. Psychol Rev 117:197

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher PW, Camerer C, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (2008) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Elsevier/Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman MS, Brown SA, Christiansen BA (1987) Expectancy theory: thinking about drinking. In: Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism, p 181

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein A (2000) Addiction: from biology to drug policy. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman M, Chaloupka FJ (1998) The demand for cocaine by young adults: a rational addiction approach. J Health Econ 17:427

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gul F, Pesendorfer W (2001) Temptation and self-control. Econometrica 69:1403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutkin BS, Dehaene S, Changeux JP (2006) A neurocomputational hypothesis for nicotine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1106

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Henly SE, Ostdiek A, Blackwell E, Knutie S, Dunlap AS, Stephens DW (2008) The discounting-by-interruptions hypothesis: model and experiment. Behav Ecol 19:154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hershberger WA (1986) An approach through the looking-glass. Anim Learn Behav 14:443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyman GM (2009) Addiction: a disorder of choice. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins ST, Heil SH, Lussier JP (2004) Clinical implications of reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders. Annu Rev Psychol 55:431

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh R (1974) The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: A theory. Behav Biol 12:421

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh R, Leber B, Gillman K (1978) Fornix fibers and motivational states as controllers of behavior: A study stimulated by the contextual retrieval theory. Behav Biol 22:463

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hu D, Amsel A (1995) A Simple Test of the Vicarious Trial-and-Error Hypothesis of Hippocampal Function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:5506

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hu D, Xu X, Gonzalez-Lima F (2006) Vicarious trial-and-error behavior and hippocampal cytochrome oxidase activity during Y-maze discrimination learning in the rat. Int J Neurosci 116:265

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt WA (1998) Pharmacology of alcohol. In: Tarter RE, Ammerman RT, Ott PJ (eds) Handbook of substance abuse: Neurobehavioral pharmacology. Plenum, New York, pp 7–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Isaacson RL (1974) The limbic system. Plenum, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Isoda M, Hikosaka O (2008) Role for subthalamic nucleus neurons in switching from automatic to controlled eye movement. J Neurosci 28:7209

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe JH, Cascella NG, Kumor KM, Sherer MA (1989) Cocaine-induced cocaine craving. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 97:59

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe A, Gitisetan S, Tarash I, Pham AZ, Jentsch JD (2010) Are nicotine-related cues susceptible to the blocking effect? Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, Program Number 268.4

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson A, Redish AD (2007) Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. J Neurosci 27:12176

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones BT, Corbin W, Fromme K (2001) A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction 96:57

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kamin LJ (1969) Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In: Learning in animals, p 279

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby KN, Herrnstein RJ (1995) Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychol Sci 6:83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruse JM, Overmier JB, Konz WA, Rokke E (1983) Pavlovian conditioned stimulus effects upon instrumental choice behavior are reinforcer specific. Learn Motiv 14:165

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhar MJ, Ritz MC, Sharkey J (1988) Cocaine receptors on dopamine transporters mediate cocaine-reinforced behavior. In: Mechanisms of cocaine abuse and toxicity, p 14

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurth-Nelson Z, Redish AD (2009) Temporal-difference reinforcement learning with distributed representations. PLoS ONE 4:e7362

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kurth-Nelson Z, Redish AD (2010) A reinforcement learning model of precommitment in decision making. Frontiers Behav Neurosci 4:184

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer EJ, Roth J (1975) Heads I win, tails it’s chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. J Pers Soc Psychol 32:951

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebron K, Milad MR, Quirk GJ (2004) Delayed recall of fear extinction in rats with lesions of ventral medial prefrontal cortex. Learn Mem 11:544

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenoir M, Serre F, Cantin L, Ahmed SH (2007) Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS ONE 2:e698

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levine AS, Billington CJ (2004) Opioids as agents of reward-related feeding: a consideration of the evidence. Physiol Behav 82:57

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liao D, Lin H, Law PY, Loh HH (2005) Mu-opioid receptors modulate the stability of dendritic spines. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:1725

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liu J-, Liu J-, Hammit JK, Chou S- (1999) The price elasticity of opium in Taiwan, 1914–1942. J Health Econ 18:795

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W (1992) Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. J Neurophysiol 67:145

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond PF (1983) Facilitation of instrumental behavior by a Pavlovian appetitive conditioned stimulus. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:225

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh NJ (1974) The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  • Madden GJ, Bickel WK (2010) Impulsivity: the behavioral and neurological science of discounting. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur J (1987) An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior, p 55

    Google Scholar 

  • McCaul ME, Petry NM (2003) The role of psychosocial treatments in pharmacotherapy for alcoholism. Am J Addict 12:S41

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFarland K, Kalivas PW (2001) The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. J Neurosci 21:8655

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milad MR, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ (2004) Electrical stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex reduces conditioned fear in a temporally specific manner. Behav Neurosci 118:389

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Montague PR, Dayan P, Sejnowski TJ (1996) A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J Neurosci 16:1936

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moos RH, Moos BS (2004) Long-term influence of duration and frequency of participation in alcoholics anonymous on individuals with alcohol use disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol 72:81

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moos RH, Moos BS (2006a) Participation in treatment and Alcoholics Anonymous: a 16-year follow-up of initially untreated individuals. J Clin Psychol 62:735

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moos RH, Moos BS (2006b) Rates and predictors of relapse after natural and treated remission from alcohol use disorders. Addiction 101:212

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muenzinger KF (1938) Vicarious trial and error at a point of choice. I. A general survey of its relation to learning efficiency. J Genet Psychol 53:75

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel L, Willner J (1980) Context and conditioning: A place for space. Physiol Psychol 8:218

    Google Scholar 

  • Nestler EJ (1996) Under siege: The brain on opiates. Neuron 16:897

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niv Y, Montague PR (2008) Theoretical and empirical studies of learning. In: Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain, p 331

    Google Scholar 

  • Niv Y, Daw ND, Dayan P (2006) Choice values. Nat Neurosci 9:987

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, Dolan RJ (2004) Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304:452

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely moving rat. Brain Res 34:171

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K (2003) Alcoholism and the brain: an overview. Alcohol Res Health 27(2):125–134

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostlund SB, Balleine BW (2008) The disunity of Pavlovian and instrumental values. Behav Brain Sci 31:456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Packard MG, McGaugh JL (1996) Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 65:65

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paine TA, Dringenberg HC, Olmstead MC (2003) Effects of chronic cocaine on impulsivity: relation to cortical serotonin mechanisms. Behav Brain Res 147:135

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Panlilio LV, Thorndike EB, Schindler CW (2007) Blocking of conditioning to a cocaine-paired stimulus: Testing the hypothesis that cocaine perpetually produces a signal of larger-than-expected reward. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86:774

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Parke J, Griffiths M (2004) Gambling addiction and the evolution of the near miss. Addict Res Theory 12:407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov I (1927) Conditioned reflexes. Oxford Univ Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips PEM, Stuber GD, Heien MLAV, Wightman RM, Carelli RM (2003) Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature 422:614

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Porrino LJ, Lyons D, Smith HR, Daunais JB, Nader MA (2004) Cocaine self-administration produces a progressive involvement of limbic, association, and sensorimotor striatal domains. J Neurosci 24:3554

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Preuschoff K, Bossaerts P, Quartz SR (2006) Neural differentiation of expected reward and risk in human subcortical structures. Neuron 51:381

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Quirk GJ, Garcia R, González-Lima F (2006) Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biol Psychiatry 60:337

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin H (2000) The science of self-control. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin H, Green L (1972) Commitment, choice, and self-control. J Exp Anal Behav 17:15

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD (1999) Beyond the cognitive map: from place cells to episodic memory. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD (2004) Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science 306:1944

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD (2009) Implications of the multiple-vulnerabilities theory of addiction for craving and relapse. Addiction 104:1940

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD, Johnson A (2007) A computational model of craving and obsession. Ann NY Acad Sci 1104:324

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD, Kurth-Nelson Z (2010) Neural models of temporal discounting. In: Impulsivity: the behavioral and neurological science of discounting, p 123

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD, Jensen S, Johnson A, Kurth-Nelson Z (2007) Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychol Rev 114:784

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redish AD, Jensen S, Johnson A (2008) A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behav Brain Sci 31:415

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla RA, Wagner AR (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II, p 64

    Google Scholar 

  • Restle F (1957) Discrimination of cues in mazes: A resolution of the ‘place-vs-response’ question. Psychol Rev 64:217

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds B, Ortengren A, Richards JB, de Wit H (2006) Dimensions of impulsive behavior: personality and behavioral measures. Pers Individ Differ 40:305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritz MC, Lamb RJ, Goldberg SR, Kuhar MJ (1987) Cocaine receptors on dopamine transporters are related to self-administration of cocaine. Science 237:1219

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brains Res Rev 18:247

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2001) Mechanisms of action of addictive stimuli: Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96:103

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2003) Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 54:25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2004) Incentive-sensitization and drug ‘wanting’. Psychopharmacology 171:352

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz W (2002) Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36:241

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague R (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schweighofer N, Shishida K, Han CE, Yamawaki S, Doya K (2006) Humans can adopt optimal discounting strategy under real-time constraints. PLoS Comput Biol 2:e152

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schweighofer N, Tanaka SC, Doya K (2007) Serotonin and the evaluation of future rewards. Theory, experiments, and possible neural mechanisms. Ann NY Acad Sci 1104:289

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Si J, Barto AG, Powell WB, Wunsch D (2004) Handbook of learning and approximate dynamic programming. Wiley/IEEE Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Simon NW, Mendez IA, Setlow B (2007) Cocaine exposure causes long-term increases in impulsive choice. Behav Neurosci 121:543

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith A, Li M, Becker S, Kapur S (2006) Dopamine, prediction error and associative learning: a model-based account. Network: Comput Neural Syst 17:61

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sotres-Bayon F, Cain CK, LeDoux JE (2006) Brain mechanisms of fear extinction: historical perspectives on the contribution of prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry 60:329

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sozou PD (1998) On hyperbolic discounting and uncertain hazard rates. R Soc Lond B 265:2015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl SM, Pradko JF, Haight BR, Modell JG, Rockett CB, Learned-Coughlin S (2004) A review of the neuropharmacology of bupropion, a dual norepinephrine and dopamine reuptake inhibitor. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiat 6:159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strotz RH (1956) Myopia and inconsistency in dynamic utility maximization. Rev Econ Stud 23:165

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton RS, Barto AG (1998) Reinforcement learning: an introduction. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmi D, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ (2008) Human Pavlovian instrumental transfer. J Neurosci 28:360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S (2004) Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci 7:887

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka SC, Schweighofer N, Asahi S, Shishida K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S, Doya K (2007) Serotonin differentially regulates short- and long-term prediction of rewards in the ventral and dorsal striatum. PLoS ONE 2:e1333

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC (1938) The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psychol Rev 45:1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC (1939) Prediction of vicarious trial and error by means of the schematic sowbug. Psychol Rev 46:318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55:189

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai HC, Zhang F, Adamantidis A, Stuber GD, Bonci A, de Lecea L, Deisseroth K (2009) Phasic firing in dopaminergic neurons is sufficient for behavioral conditioning. Science 324:1080

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Uslaner JM, Acerbo MJ, Jones SA, Robinson TE (2006) The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine. Behav Brain Res 169:320

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer MA, Redish AD (2009) Covert expectation-of-reward in rat ventral striatum at decision points. Frontiers Integr Neurosci 3:1

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer MA, Redish AD (2010) Expectancies in decision making, reinforcement learning, and ventral striatum. Front Neurosci 4:29

    Google Scholar 

  • Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W (2001) Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature 412:43

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenaar WA (1988) Paradoxes of gambling behavior. Erlbaum, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner I, Lubow RE, Feldon J (1988) Disruption of latent inhibition by acute administration of low doses of amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 30:871

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • White AM (2003) What happened? Alcohol, memory blackouts, and the brain. Alcohol Res Health 27(2):186–196

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yin HH, Knowlton B, Balleine BW (2004) Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci 19:181

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu AJ, Dayan P (2005) Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron 46:681

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to A. David Redish .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kurth-Nelson, Z., Redish, A.D. (2012). Modeling Decision-Making Systems in Addiction. In: Gutkin, B., Ahmed, S. (eds) Computational Neuroscience of Drug Addiction. Springer Series in Computational Neuroscience, vol 10. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-0751-5_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics