Skip to main content

Neuro-Cognitive Systems Involved in Moral Reasoning

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Moral Brain

Before considering the systems involved in moral reasoning, it is worth briefly considering what morality is. Two broad viewpoints on morality effectively exist. According to one, moral reasoning is mediated by some form of unitary system for processing social rule related information. Early adherents of this viewpoint included Kohlberg who considered the moral reasoning developed from conventional reasoning (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987). More recent adherents include Moll who has defined morality as: “the sets of customs and values that are embraced by a cultural group to guide social conduct” (Moll, Zahn, Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005, p. 799).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

References

  • Aniskiewicz, A. S. (1979). Autonomic components of vicarious conditioning and psychopathy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 60–67.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J. B., & Nunner-Winkler, G. (1992). Children’s moral motive strength and temperamental inhibition reduce their immoral behaviour in real moral conflicts. Child Development, 63, 1223–1235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Fultz, J. & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Adults’ emotional reactions to the distress of others. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 163–185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., & Webb, S. (2005). Individuals with autism spectrum disorder show normal responses to a fear potential startle paradigm. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beer, J. S., Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2003). The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: Insights from patients with orbitofrontal damage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 594–604.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz, S., Armony, J., Blair, R. J. R., & Dolan, R. (2002). Neural correlates of violation of social norms and embarrassment. Brain, 125, 1696–1708.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Grodd, W., et al. (2005). Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 799–805.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, K. S., Marsh, A. A., Morton, J., Vythilingham, M., Jones, M., Mondillo, K., et al. (2006). Choosing the lesser of two evils, the better of two goods: Specifying the roles of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in object choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 11379–11386.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, K. S., Smith, B. W., Mitchell, D. G., Morton, J., Vythilingam, M., Pessoa, L., et al. (2007). Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion. NeuroImage, 35(1), 430–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abell, F. & Cipolotti, L. (2002). Fractionation of visual memory: agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 108–118.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (1996). Brief report: Morality in the autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 571–579.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (1997). Moral reasoning in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 731–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2003). A neurocognitive model of the psychopathic individual. In M. A. Ron & T. W. Robbins (Eds.), Disorders of brain and mind 2 (pp. 400–420). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and Cognition, 55, 198–208.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., & Cipolotti, L. (2000). Impaired social response reversal: A case of “acquired sociopathy”. Brain, 123, 1122–1141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 491–498.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1997). The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology, 34, 192–198.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Marsh, A. A., Finger, E., Blair, K. S., & Luo, Q. (2006). Neuro-cognitive systems involved in morality. Philosophical Explorations, 9, 13–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain, 122, 883–893.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, J. S., Hynes, C., Van Horn, J., Grafton, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: an FMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 803–817.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bormann-Kischkel, C., Vilsmeier, M., & Baude, B. (1995). The development of emotional concepts in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1243–1259.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 539–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, 843–859.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brody, G. H., & Shaffer, D. R. (1982). Contributions of parents and peers to children’s moral socialisation. Developmental Review, 2, 31–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Budhani, S., Marsh, A. A., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of response reversal: Considering acquisition. NeuroImage, 34, 1754–1765.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1959). Emotional reactions of rats to the pain of others. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 52, 132–134.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgement. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cubero, I., Thiele, T. E., & Bernstein, I. L. (1999). Insular cortex lesions and taste aversion learning: Effects of conditioning method and timing of lesion. Brain Research, 839(2), 323–330.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D. J., Merz, S., Riddell, A. C., Haines, D. J., et al. (2006). Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 280–281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality and the human brain. New York: Putnam (Grosset Books) .

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behaviour caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research 41, 81–94.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, M. (1991), Living with Asperger’s syndrome. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger’s syndrome (pp. 184–206). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2006). Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1563–1569.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, R. J., & Vuilleumier, P. (2003). Amygdala automaticity in emotional processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 348–355.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, D. D., Shin, L. M., Alpert, N. M., Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Lasko, M., et al. (1999). Anger in healthy men: A PET study using script-driven imagery. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 466–472.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Everitt, B. J., Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Appetitive behavior: Impact of amygdala-dependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Annual New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 233–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307–314.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feshbach, N. D. (1987). Parental empathy and child adjustment/ maladjustment. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Kamel, N., Mitchell, D. G., & Blair, J. R. (2006). Caught in the act: The impact of audience on the neural response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior. NeuroImage, 33, 414–421.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50, 531–534.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happe, F. (2005). Autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15, R786–R790.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2007). Differential fear conditioning in Asperger’s syndrome: Implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2125–2134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, P. (1997). The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 113–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, J. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Human orbitofrontal cortex mediates extinction learning while accessing conditioned representations of value. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1144–1152.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44, 389–400.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 1971–1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on the child’s internalization of values: A reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30, 4–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 31, 998–1002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, Issue on Human Nature, 135(4), 55–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harenski, C. L., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. NeuroImage, 30, 313–324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heekeren, H. R., Wartenburger, I., Schmidt, H., Prehn, K., Schwintowski, H. P., & Villringer, A. (2005). Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making. NeuroImage, 24, 887–897.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heekeren, H. R., Wartenburger, I., Schmidt, H., Schwintowski, H. P., & Villringer, A. (2003). An fMRI study of simple ethical decision-making. Neuroreport, 14, 1215–1219.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Conscience, personality and socialiszation techniques. Human Development, 13, 90–126.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, M. A., Cowell, P. E., Boucher, J., Broks, P., Mayes, A., Farrant, A., et al. (2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism. Neuroreport, 11, 1931–1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G. L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 685–695.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987). The emergence of morality in young children University of Chicago Press: Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., & Anderson, C. (2000). Saving face for Darwin: The functions and uses of embarrassment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 187–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250–270.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 29, 499–508.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Knobe, J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis, 63, 190–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Adams, C. M., Varner, J. L., & Hommer, D. (2001). Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport, 12, 3683–3687.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., De Vet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putman, P. (1994). Maternal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children. Child Development, 65, 852–868.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., Landel, J. L., & Patton, K. M. (1996). The motivated expression of embarrassment following a self-presentational predicament. Journal of Personality, 64, 619–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., & Meadows, S. (1991). Predictors, elicitors, and concomitants of social blushing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 254–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1998). The Emotional Brain. New York: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leibenluft, E., Blair, R. J., Charney, D. S., & Pine, D. S. (2003). Irritability in pediatric mania and other childhood psychopathology. Annual New York Academy of Sciences, 1008, 201–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., Knobe, J., & Cohen, A. (2006). Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect. Psychological Science, 17, 421–427.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., Mallon, R., & DiCorcia, J. A. (2006). Transgressors, victims and cry babies: Is basic moral judgment spared in autism? Social Neuroscience, 1, 270–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lissek, S., Powers, A. S., McClure, E. B., Phelps, E. A., Woldehawariat, G., Grillon, C., et al. (2005). Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1391–1424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lough, S., Kipps, C. M., Treise, C., Watson, P., Blair, J. R. & Hodges, J. R. (2006). Social reasoning, emotion and empathy in frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 950–958.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, Q., Nakic, M., Wheatley, T., Richell, R., Martin, A., & Blair, R. J. (2006). The neural basis of implicit moral attitude--an IAT study using event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 30, 1449–1457.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masserman, J. H., Wechkin, S. & Terris, W. (1964). “Altruistic” behavior in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry, 121.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E. B., Treland, J. E., Snow, J., Schmajuk, M., Dickstein, D. P., Towbin, K. E., et al. (2005). Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1644–1651.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: it’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. G., Nakic, M., Fridberg, D., Kamel, N., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. (2007). The impact of processing load on emotion. NeuroImage, 34, 1299–1309.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16, 696–703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., et al. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2730–2736.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignacio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Daquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 68–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Zahn, R., Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2005). Opinion: The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 799–809.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Montagne, B., van Honk, J., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., et al. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 5–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. (2002). Norms with feeling: Towards a psychological account of moral judgment. Cognition, 84, 221–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. (2004). Sentimental rules: On the natural foundations of moral judgment. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L., Turiel, E., & Encarnacion-Gawrych, G. E. (1983). Social interactions and social concepts: Analysis of morality and convention in the Virgin Islands. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 14, 469–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P. (1982). Conceptual development in the moral and conventional domains: Implications for values education. Review of Educational Research, 52, 93–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P., & Nucci, M. (1982). Children’s social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53, 403–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschollo children. Child Development, 49, 400–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oxford, M., Cavell, T. A., & Hughes, J. N. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits moderate the relation between ineffective parenting and child externalizing problems: A partial replication and extension. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 577–585.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M. P., Hozack, N., Frank, L., & Brown, G. G. (2002). Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. NeuroImage, 15, 836.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States America, 99, 11458–11463.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., Padmala, S,. & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. NeuroImage, 28, 249–255.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Scott, S. K., Calder, A. J., Andrew, C., Giampietro, V., et al. (1998). Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 265, 1809–1817.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrews, C., Calder, A. J., et al. (1997). A specified neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–498.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1932). The moral development of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 203–213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, G. E. (1965). Aiding responses in rats: Not in guinea pigs. Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 73, 105–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, G. E., & Gainer, P. (1962). “Altruism” in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 55, 123–125.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, D., Snarey, J., Ousley, O., Harenski, K., DuBois Bowman, F., Gilkey, R., et al. (2007). The neural processing of moral sensitivity to issues of justice and care. Neuropsychologia, 45, 755–766.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, J., Viding, E., James Blair, R., Frith, U., & Happe, F. (2006). Autism spectrum disorder and psychopathy: Shared cognitive underpinnings or double hit? Psychological Medicine, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., Ramnani, N., Mackay, C., Wilson, J. L., Jezzard, P., Carter, C. S., et al. (2004). Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 594.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., Ahadi, S., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament an social behaviour in children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In (J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 1–83).Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. G. (1981). Preschool children’s conceptions of moral and social rules. Child Development, 52, 1333–1336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. G. (1993). Understanding of social rules. In M. Bennett (Ed.), The child as psychologist: An introduction to the development of social cognition (pp. 111–141). New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, M., Smetana, J. G., & Kim, S. Y. (1987). Korean children’s conceptions of moral and conventional transgressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 577–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprengelmeyer, R., Rausch, M., Eysel, U. T., & Przuntek, H. (1998). Neural structures associated with the recognition of facial basic emotions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.B, 265, 1927–1931.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, S., Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Brief report: Developmental change in theory of mind abilities in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 461–467.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trasler, G. (1978). Relations between psychopathy and persistent criminality – methodological and theoretical issues. In R. D. Hare & D. Schalling (Eds.), Psychopathic behaviour: Approaches to research.Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E., Killen, M., & Helwig, C. C. (1987). Morality: Its structure, functions, and vagaries. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 155–245). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentin, V. V., Dickinson, A., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 4019–4026.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16, 780–784.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997). Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: The moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 292–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 8235–8240.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Helwig, C. C., & Lau, A. (1996). Intention, act, and outcome in behavioural prediction and moral judgment. Child Development, 67, 2478–2492.

    Google Scholar 

Jorge Moll’s Response on James Blair’s Paper

  • In his provocative chapter, Blair puts forth a “multiple moralities” approach, a view he shares with Jon Haidt and others, and contrasts it to what he interpreted from our work to be “a unitary view of morality”. His criticisms, although well articulated, simply do not do apply to our research, less so to our theoretical premises, as we will attempt to briefly show below.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firstly, we have never claimed that that we viewed morality as “unitary”, either phenomenologically or in terms of brain mechanisms, less so that this was in opposition to any purported multiple model of morality. Our early reference to Colby, Kohlberg, Speicher, Hewer, Candee, and Gibss’s work (Moll, Eslinger, & Oliveira-Souza, 2001) was a matter of borrowing an operational definition of moral judgment to start with, and cannot be taken as an argument to classify our views as “rationalistic” – even if that was the only experimental work ever carried out by our group. As often happens with flourishing lines of research, one regularly finds himself straddling on uncharted grounds, and must draw first on very general hypotheses. As far as we’re aware, we were among the first to study the functional correlates of moral judgment with fMRI (Oliveira-Souza & Moll, 2000), and at that time we necessarily had to depart from general ideas. In our view, considering this methodological strategy to be a solid theoretical system of premises is an undue overinterpretation of our work.

    Google Scholar 

  • Although we would refrain from using the expression “multiple moralities” (for reasons which fall beyond the scope of this short reply), we see no contradiction at all between Blair’s views of multiple processes underlying morality and our own perspective on morality. While exposing his arguments, Blair contrasts his “multiple moralities” view to a “unitary morality” and refers to the following definition employed in our theoretical article (Moll, Zahn, Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005), which was taken from MacIntyre’s After Virtue (1984): “Here, morality is considered as the sets of customs and values that are embraced by a cultural group to guide social conduct, a view that does not assume the existence of absolute moral values”. Once more, the fact that we have used an operational definition to delimit what we consider to be the moral domain sphere in no way should be taken to imply that we have adopted a “unitary” stance regarding either the phenomenology or the neural underpinnings of morality. On the contrary, in our model paper we explicitly describe how different types of impairment of moral conduct do emerge from dysfunction in discrete brain regions, and provide clear hypotheses on how damage to subcomponents of the various cortical and limbic regions that have been linked to moral cognition and behavior would lead to distinct types of moral dysfunction. Assuming that we believe in a “unitary morality” because we have pointed to the involvement of a broadly defined but stable neural architecture in diverse aspects of moral cognition and behavior is therefore a misinterpretation of our theoretical and experimental work. This would be equivalent to claim that if one believes that visual function, broadly speaking, relies on a well defined set of brain regions (the visual system), one would necessarily believe that all distinctive visual experiences (e.g., motion, color, space) rely on exactly the same neural substrates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Secondly, Blair’s claim that we favor rationalistic views in moral psychology is also surprising. We were among the first to recognize and experimentally explore the pivotal role of emotions both in moral judgment and in moral sensitivity – and this was in fact largely influenced by the work of Jon Haidt on moral and basic emotions. Our views on how emotions are critically linked to moral appraisals were articulated in two papers in which we devoted particular attention to the influence of distinct moral emotions in moral judgment (Moll, Oliveira-Souza, Bramati, & Grafman, 2002; Moll, Oliveira-Souza, Eslinger, et al., 2002), a fact that is overlooked in Blair’s chapter. Furthermore, it is also interesting that Blair dedicates considerable space in his chapter discussing disgust as evidence for his multiple moralities view, and yet he fails to acknowledge our fMRI study on moral disgust, which was actually the first to directly probe the contribution of basic disgust and moral disgust to brain activation (Moll, Oliveira-Souza, et al., 2005). 

    Google Scholar 

  • Finally, James Blair unfortunately did not have the opportunity to assess our latest views on the contribution of attachment to morality, another piece of work which clearly reflects our view on how distinct neurobiological components and functions support diverse aspects of human morality.

    Google Scholar 

  • In summary, Blair’s qualification of “unitary moral system” is not appropriately applicable to our work. As such, our work is not actually a good framework against which to build criticisms in support of multiple moralities. If anything, were our work useful for the purpose of contrasting unitary versus multiple moralities, it would probably fit much more naturally as evidence in support of, not against, the latter view.

    Google Scholar 

References

  • Daquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust – A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(1), 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. (1984). After virtue: A study in moral theory (2nd ed.). Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16(3), 696–703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., et al. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(7), 2730–2736.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignacio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Daquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(1), 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Eslinger, P. J., & Oliveira-Souza, R. (2001). Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task – Preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects. Arquivos De Neuro-Psiquiatria, 59(3B), 657–664.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Zahn, R., Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2005). The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 799–809.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveira-Souza, R., & Moll, J. (2000). The moral brain: A functional MRI study of moral judgment. Neurology, 54(7), A104–A104.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health: National Institute of Mental Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James Blair .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Blair, J. (2009). Neuro-Cognitive Systems Involved in Moral Reasoning. In: Verplaetse, J., Schrijver, J., Vanneste, S., Braeckman, J. (eds) The Moral Brain. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6287-2_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics