Skip to main content

The Regulative Dimension of Folk Psychology

  • Chapter

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bates E et al. (1975) The acquisition of preformatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Q 21:205–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff M et al. (eds) (2002) The cognitive animal. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilgrami A (1998) Self-knowledge and resentment. In: Wright C, Smith B, Macdonald C (eds) Knowing our own minds. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton TB, Tronick E (1980) Preverbal communication between mothers and infants. In: Olson DR (ed) The social foundations of language and thought. Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne D (2004) Do dolphins know their own minds? Biol Philos 19(4):633–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner J (1983) Child’s talk: learning to use language. Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner J (1990) Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RW, Whiten A (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campos JJ, Sternberg CR (1981) Perception appraisal and emotion: the onset of social referencing. In: Lamb ME, Sherrod LR (eds) Infant social cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M et al. (1998) Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behav Dev 21(2):315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1992) Characterizing the mind of another species. Behav Brain Sci 15:172–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland P (1979) Scientific realism and the plasticity of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie G, Ravenscroft I (2003). Recreative minds. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson D (1984) Inquiries into truth & interpretation. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies M (1994) The mental simulation debate. In: Peacocke C (ed) Objectivity, simulation and the unity of consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1978) Brainstorms. Bradford Press, Montgomery, VT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1991) Real patterns. J Philos:27–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1987) Psychosemantics. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel H (1967) Studies in ethnomethodology. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G et al. (1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56(2):165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergen KJ (1982) Toward transformation in social knowledge. Springer, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman E (1959) The presentation of self in everyday life. Anchor Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A (1989) Interpretation psychologized. Mind Lang 4:161–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A (1992) In defense of the simulation theory. Mind Lang 7(1/2):104–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman AI (1993) The psychology of folk-psychology. Behav Brain Sci 16:15–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A et al. (2000) Early theories of mind: what the theory theory can tell us about autism. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (eds) Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 50–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon R (1986) Folk psychology as simulation. Mind Lang 1:158–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon R (1992) The simulation theory: objections and misconceptions. Mind Lang 17:11–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heal J (1998) Co-cognition and off-line simulation: two ways off understanding the simulation approach. Mind Lang 13(4):477–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heal J (2003) Mindreading: an integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Mind 114(453):181–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider F, Simmel M (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am J Psychol 57:243–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes C (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21:101–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto D (1999) The presence of mind. Benjamins, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto D (2004) The limits of spectatorial folk-psychology. Mind Lang 19(5):548–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto D (2007) Folk-psychology without theory or simulation. In: Hutto D, Ratcliffe M (eds) Flok psychology re-assessed. pp 115–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson F (1992) Block’s challenge. In: Campbell K, Bacon J, Rhinehart L (eds) Ontology, causality, and mind: essays on the philosophy of David Armstrong. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson F, Pettit P (1990a) In defence of folk psychology. Philos Stud 57:7–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson SC (2000) The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy. Trends Cogn Sci 4(1):22–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson S et al. (1998) Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Dev Sci 1(2):233–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaye K (1982) The mental and social life of babies: how parents create persons. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis D (1983) Philosophical papers, vol 1. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein G, Adler D (1995). A bias in the prediction of tastes. Econ J 105:929–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGeer V (1996) Is “self-knowledge” an empirical problem? Renegotiating the space of philosophical explanation. J Philos 93:483–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGeer V (2001) Psycho-practice, psycho-theory and the contrastive case of autism. J Conscious Stud 8(5–7):109–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeer V, Pettit P (2002). The self-regulating mind. Lang Commun 22(3):281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN (1995) Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev Psychol 31: 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Gopnik A (1993) The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (eds) Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 335–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1983) Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev 54(3):702–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1994) Imitation, memory and the representation of persons. Infant Behav Dev 17(1):83–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1997) Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev Parent 6:179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan RG (1993) White Queen psychology and other essays for Alice. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton A (1980) Frames of mind: constraints on the commonsense conception of the mental. Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols S (2003) Folk psychology. In: Nadel L (ed) Encyclopedia of cognitive science. Nature, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg IM (1999) The Alex studies: cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettit P (1978) Rational man theory. In: Hookway C, Pettit P (eds) Action and interpretation: studies in the philosophy of the social sciences. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 43–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettit P (1993) The common mind: an essay on psychology, society and politics. Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli D (1996) Chimpanzee theory of mind? The long road to strong inference. In: Carruthers P, Smith P (eds) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli D, Eddy TJ (1996) What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 61(3):1–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 4:515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repacholi BM (1998) Infants’ use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person’s emotional expression. Dev Psychol 34:1017–1025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryle G (1949) The concept of mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellars W (1997) Empiricism and the philosophy of mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson P (1974) Freedom and resentment. Freedom and resentment and other essays. Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson JS (1979) Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In: Thoman EG (ed) Origin’s of the infant’s social responsiveness. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 33–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1958) Philosophical investigations. Blackwell, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2007 springer

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

McGeer, V. (2007). The Regulative Dimension of Folk Psychology. In: Hutto, D.D., Ratcliffe, M. (eds) Folk Psychology Re-Assessed. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-5558-4_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics