Skip to main content

Somatovisceral Activation During Anger

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
International Handbook of Anger

Abstract

What is the physiological signature of anger? If not anger, which emotion would be more destined to turn a “cold” object perception into a Jamesian “hot” emotional encounter? Indeed, reports of bodily anger sensations are descriptions of heat and tension. However, the message from studies reporting physiological anger responses is more difficult to reconcile. The chapter discusses landmark studies on the differentiation between anger and fear. It is emphasized that their methodological characteristics are decisive for demonstrating or failing to show physiological anger specificity. A meta-analysis shows that anger provocation elicits strong changes in systolic and diastolic blood pressure, heart rate, number of skin conductance responses, and muscle activity. The pattern resembles the combined action of adrenaline and noradrenaline, accompanied by strong vagal withdrawal. It is argued that these coordinated changes have a functional value for the pursuit and finally the attainment of the goal of anger: to motivate individuals to avoid failure and pain by averting subordination under physically or socially caused harm and to gain superiority.

This work has been supported by grants Ste405 of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Adrenaline stimulates alpha-adrenergic, beta1- and beta2-adrenergic receptors. Adrenergic responses are defined as reductions in finger temperature (probably because blood is redistributed from the skin to skeletal muscles), diastolic blood pressure, and total peripheral resistance and as increases in heart rate, systolic blood pressure, stroke volume, left-ventricular contractility, cardiac output, number of skin conductance responses, and respiration rate. Noradrenaline stimulates alpha- and beta1-adrenergic receptors. Noradrenergic responses are characterized by increases in systolic and diastolic blood pressure, left-ventricular contractility, number of skin conductance responses, and total peripheral resistance, as well as reductions in heart rate and finger temperature (Chessick et al., 1966; Löllgen, Meuret, Just, & Wiemers, 1985; Wenger et al., 1960). Compared to adrenaline, noradrenaline produces a lower heart and respiration rate, lower stroke volume and cardiac output, but a higher diastolic blood pressure and total peripheral resistance and a higher finger temperature.

  2. 2.

    In a crossed carry-over design and randomized across sessions, subjects were given either Placebo, partial beta-adrenergic and cholinergic blockade (“alpha-free”), partial alpha-adrenergic and cholinergic blockade (“beta-free”), or partial alpha- and beta-adrenergic blockade (“chol-free”).

References

  • Averill, J. R. (1974). An analysis of psychophysiological symbolism and its influence on theories of emotion. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 4, 147–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ax, A. F. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 28–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., Sarter, M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Ascending visceral regulation of cortical affective information processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(8), 2103–2109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buell, J. C., Alpert, B. S., & McCrory, W. W. (1986). Physical stressors as elicitors of cardiovascular reactivity. In K. A. Matthews, S. M. Weiss, T. Detre, T. M. Dembrowski, B. Falkner, S. B. Manuck, & R. B. Williams (Eds.), Handbook of stress, reactivity, and cardiovascular disease (pp. 127–144). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Klein, D. J. (1992). What is an emotion? The role of somatovisceral afference, with special emphasis on somatovisceral "illusions". In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior. Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 63–98). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chessick, R. D., Bassan, M., & Shattan, S. (1966). A comparison of the effect of infused catecholamines and certain affect states. American Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 156–165.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cobos, P., Sanchez, M., Garcia, C., Vera, M. N., & Vila, J. (2002). Revisiting the James versus Cannon debate on emotion: Startle and autonomic modulation in patients with spinal cord injuries. Biological Psychology, 61, 251–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., Josephs, O., O‘Doherty, J., Zanini, S., Dewar, B. K., et al. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain, 126, 2139–2152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 189–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duschek, S., Meinhardt, J., & Schandry, R. (2006). Reduced cortical activity due to chronic low blood pressure: An EEG study. Biological Psychology, 72(3), 241–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C. (1994). William James and emotion: Is a century of fame worth a century of misunderstanding? Psychological Review, 101(2), 222–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elmadjian, F., Hope, J. M., & Freeman, H. (1957). Methacholine test and epinephrine and arterenol excretion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 77, 399–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdmann, G., & Baumann, S. (1996). Sind psychophysiologische Veränderungen im Paradigma "Öffentliches Sprechen" Ausdruck emotionaler Belastung? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 43(2), 224–255.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J. (1987). Theory in psychophysiology: The multi-component analysis of psychophysiological reactivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 1, 9–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, F. S., & Stern, J. A. (1970). Peripheral physiological variables and emotion: The James–Lange theory revisited. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 411–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foerster, F. (1985). Psychophysiological response specificities: A replication over a 12-month period. Biological Psychology, 21, 169–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foerster, F., Myrtek, M., & Stemmler, G. (1993). Reactivity to multiple stressors: A course in synergism. Journal of Psychophysiology, 7(2), 115–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhaeuser, M. (1979). Psychoneuroendocrine approaches to the study of emotion as related to stress and coping. In R. A. Dienstbier (Ed.), 1978 Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 123–161). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funkenstein, D. H. (1956). Nor-epinephrine-like and epinephrine-like substances in relation to human behavior. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 124, 58–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funkenstein, D. H., King, S. H., & Drolette, M. (1954). The direction of anger during a laboratory stress-inducing situation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 16, 404–413.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glass, D. C., Krakoff, L. R., Contrada, R., Hilton, W. F., Kehoe, K., Mannucci, E. G., et al. (1980). Effect of harassment and competition upon cardiovascular and plasma catecholamine responses in Type A and Type B individuals. Psychophysiology, 17, 453–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilton, S. M. (1982). The defence-arousal system and its relevance for circulatory and respiratory control. Journal of Experimental Biology, 100, 159–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is emotion? Mind, 19, 188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion. Psychological Review, 1, 516–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jänig, W. (2003). The autonomic nervous system and its co-ordination by the brain. In R. J. Davidson, H. H. Goldsmith, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 135–186). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jänig, W. (2006). The integrative action of the autonomic nervous system: Neurobiology of homeostasis. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Janke, B. (2002). Entwicklung des Emotionswissens bei Kindern. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoebel, S. B., McHenry, P. L., Phillips, J. F., & Widlansky, S. (1974). Atropine-induced cardioacceleration and myocardial blood flow in subjects with and without coronary artery disease. American Journal of Cardiology, 33, 327–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I., Bateman, D. E., & van Lehn, R. (1953). Autonomic response specificity: An experimental study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 8–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. A., & Leenen, F. H. (1989). Role of beta 1-receptors and vagal tone in cardiac inotropic and chronotropic responses to a beta 2-agonist in humans. Circulation, 79, 107–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Löllgen, H., Meuret, G., Just, H., & Wiemers, K. (1985). Sympathikomimetika in der Notfall- und Intensivmedizin. Deutsches Ärzteblatt, 82, 1951–1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mäntysaari, M. J., Antila, K. J., & Peltonen, T. E. (1988). Circulatory effects of anticipation in a light isometric handgrip test. Psychophysiology, 25, 179–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. E., Shaver, J. A., Leon, D. F., Thompson, M. E., Reddy, P. S., & Leonard, J. J. (1974). Autonomic mechanisms in hemodynamic responses to isometric exercise. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 54, 104–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myrtek, M., & Spital, S. (1986). Psychophysiological response patterns to single, double, and triple stressors. Psychophysiology, 23, 663–671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, G. I., Silke, B., Hussain, M., Verma, S. P., & Taylor, S. H. (1984). Rest and exercise hemodynamic effects of sequential alpha-1-adrenoceptor (trimazosin) and beta-adrenoceptor (propranolol) antagonism in essential hypertension. American Heart Journal, 108, 124–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicotra, A., Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Emotional and autonomic consequences of spinal cord injury explored using functional brain imaging. Brain, 129, 718–728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 315–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2007). Neurologizing the psychology of affects: How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 281–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion – A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollatos, O., Gramann, K., & Schandry, R. (2007). Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Human Brain Mapping, 28(1), 9–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rimé, B., Philippot, P., & Cisamolo, D. (1990). Social schemata of peripheral changes in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 38–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ring, C., Edwards, L., & Kavussanu, M. (2008). Effects of isometric exercise on pain are mediated by blood pressure. Biological Psychology, 78(1), 123–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, J. (1957). Pain, fear, and anger in hypertensives and normotensives. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19, 17–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S. (1975). Cognition and peripheralist – centralist controversies in motivation and emotion. In M. S. Gazzaniga & C. Blakemore (Eds.), Handbook of psychobiology (pp. 529–564). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, R. G. (1984). The physiological role of alpha- and beta-adrenoceptors in the regional circulation. In W. Kobinger & R. P. Ahlquist (Eds.), Alpha and beta adrenoceptors and the cardiovascular system (pp. 109–123). Princeton, NJ: Excerpta Medica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silke, B., Nelson, G. I., Ahuja, R. C., Okoli, R. C., & Taylor, S. H. (1983). Comparative haemodynamic dose-response effects of intravenous propranolol and pindolol in patients with coronary heart disease. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 25, 157–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, R., Lovallo, W. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1992). Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 422–435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, O. A., DeVito, J. L., & Astley, C. A. (1990). Neurons controlling cardiovascular responses to emotion are located in lateral hypothalamus-perifornical region. American Journal of Physiology, 259, R943–R954.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (1984). Psychophysiologische Emotionsmuster. Frankfurt: Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (1989). The autonomic differentiation of emotions revisited: Convergent and discriminant validation. Psychophysiology, 26, 617–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (1992a). Differential psychophysiology: Persons in situations. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (1992b). The vagueness of specificity: Models of peripheral physiological emotion specificity in emotion theories and their experimental discriminability. Journal of Psychophysiology, 6(1), 17–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (2003). Methodological considerations in the psychophysiological study of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, H. H. Goldsmith, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 225–255). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G. (2004). Physiological processes during emotion. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 33–70). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G., & Fahrenberg, J. (1989). Psychophysiological assessment: Conceptual, psychometric, and statistical issues. In G. Turpin (Ed.), Handbook of clinical psychophysiology (pp. 71–104). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. A., & Scherer, T. (2001). Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: The context counts. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 275–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, J. F., & Brosschot, J. F. (2005). Psychosomatics and psychopathology: looking up and down from the brain. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 1050–1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, H. (1989). The peripheral physiological differentiation of emotions. In H. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social psychophysiology (pp. 77–98). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenger, M. A., Clemens, T. L., Darsie, M. L., Engel, B. T., Estess, F. M., & Sonnenschein, R. R. (1960). Autonomic response patterns during intravenous infusion of epinephrine and nor-epinephrine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 22, 294–307.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18, 442–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, M. G., Lake, C. R., & Kopin, I. J. (1977). The sympathetic-nervous-system defect in primary orthostatic hypotension. New England Journal of Medicine, 296, 293–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerhard Stemmler .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Stemmler, G. (2010). Somatovisceral Activation During Anger. In: Potegal, M., Stemmler, G., Spielberger, C. (eds) International Handbook of Anger. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-89676-2_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics