Skip to main content

Abstract

Diagrams for presenting quantitative data are an important component of print communication. Their rate of use is high and rising. This reflects in part the recent development of software tools for generating data graphics. These programs allow a wide range of choices for data visualisation — some of which may be ugly or ineffective. How has graph usage evolved during this period? A survey of graph usage in academic journals, magazines, and newspapers during the years 1985-1994 revealed several dynamic trends in the characteristics of data graphics, as well as robust differences between media. However, graph features that have been singled out by experts as poor choices, such as “3-D” rendering, do not seem to be on the rise.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adrenaline Software, Inc. (1993). [Computer software]. Charts Pro 1.04. Québec, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Bertin, J. (1967). Sémiologie graphique: Les diagrammes, les réseaux, les cartes. Paris: La Haye Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bertin, J. (1980). The basic test of the graph: A matrix theory of graph construction and cartography. In P.A. Kolers, M.E. Wrolstad and H. Bouma (Eds), Processing of visible language 2. New York:Plenum Press, pp. 585–604.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bertin, J. (1983). Semiology of graphics (W.J. Berg, trans.). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Carswell, C.M. (1992). Choosing specifiers: An evaluation of the basic tasks model of graphical perception. Special issue: Visual displays. Human Factors 34:535–554.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Casali, J.G. and Gaylin, K.B. (1988). Selected graph design variables in four interpretation tasks: A microcomputer-based pilot study. Behaviour & Information Technology 7:31–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Clark, H.H. and Clark, E.V. (1977). Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Cleveland, W.S. (1984). Graphs in scientific publications. American Statistician 38(4):261–269

    Google Scholar 

  9. Cleveland, W.S. (1985). The elements of graphing data. Monterey, CA: Wadsworth Advanced Books and Software.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Cleveland, W.S., Harris, C.S. and McGill, R. (1983). Experiments on quantitative judgments of graphs and maps. Bell System Technical Journal 62:16591674.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Fish, D. and McCartney, R., this volume.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Gattis, M. and Holyoak, K.J. (1996). Mapping conceptual to spatial relations in visual reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22:231–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Howard, I.P. (1982). Human visual orientation. New York:Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Kerpedjiev, S., this volume.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Kerpedjiev, S., Carenini, G., Green, N., Moore, J. and Roth, S. (1998, October). Saying it in graphics: From intentions to visualisations. Paper presented at the EEE symposium on information visualisation (InfoVis ’98), Research Triangle Park, NC.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Kosslyn, S. (1993). Elements of graph design. New York:Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Larkin, J.H. and Simon, H.A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science 11:65–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Lohse, G.L., Walker, N. and Rueler, H.H. (1994). A classification of visual representations. Communications of the ACM 37:36–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Macdonald-Ross, M. (1977). How numbers are shown. AV Communications Review 25:359–409.

    Google Scholar 

  21. MathSoft (1995). [Computer software]. S-Plus 3.3. Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Pinker, S. (1990). A Theory of graph comprehension. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Artificial intelligence and the future of testing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 73–126.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Shah, P. and Carpenter, P.A. (1995). Conceptual limitations in comprehending line graphs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124:43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Simkin, D. and Hastie, R. (1987). An information-processing analysis of graph perception. Journal of the American Statistical Association 82:454–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Spence, I. (1990). Visual psychophysics of simple graphical elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:683–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Tufte, E.R. (1983). The visual display of quantitative information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Tufte, E.R. (1990). Envisioning information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Tukey, J.W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA:Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Tversky, B. (1995). Cognitive origins of graphic conventions. In F.T. Marchese (Ed.), Understanding images. New York: Springer, pp. 29–53.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  30. Tversky, B. and Schiano, D.J. (1989). Perceptual and conceptual factors in distortions in memory for graphs and maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118:387–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Wainer, H. (1984). How to display data badly. American Statistician 38:137–147.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Zacks, J., Levy, E., Tversky, B. and Schiano, D.J. (1998). Reading bar graphs: Effects of extraneous depth cues and graphical context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 4:119–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Zacks, J. and Tversky, B. (1999). Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication. Memory and Cognition 27(6):1073–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer-Verlag London

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zacks, J., Levy, E., Tversky, B., Schiano, D. (2002). Graphs in Print. In: Anderson, M., Meyer, B., Olivier, P. (eds) Diagrammatic Representation and Reasoning. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-0109-3_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-0109-3_11

  • Publisher Name: Springer, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-85233-242-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4471-0109-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics