Considerable work has shown changes in cognition as people age. Most typically, there are reductions in memory, executive functions, and processing speed in older populations (Grady & Craik, 2000; Rabbitt, 1997; Salthouse, 2009), though more “crystallized” functions such as language tend to be preserved (Rabbitt, 1997). Changes in social cognitive functions as people age have been studied less extensively. For example, there are reports that older people have more difficulty seeing other people’s perspective in “theory of mind” (ToM) tasks (Slessor, Phillips, & Bull, 2007, 2008). Older adults are also more affected by self-related stimuli than younger participants, when asked to memorize material (Mather & Carstensen, 2005; Trelle, Henson, & Simons, 2015), consistent with lay reports that older people become more self-absorbed and inward-looking. These enhancing effects of self bias are found even when comparisons are made under conditions in which the stimuli are semantically encoded (Trelle et al., 2015), suggesting that the effects do not simply reflect the depth of processing. The reasons for these changes are unclear, however. For example, performance in ToM tasks is known to be influenced by executive capacity (Apperly, Samson, Chiavarino, & Humphreys, 2004), so the reduced ToM performance in older adults may reflect less executive control (Cavallini et al., 2015; Gutchess, Kensinger, & Schacter, 2010; Henry, Phillips, Ruffman, & Bailey, 2013; Shamay-Tsoory & Aharon-Peretz, 2007). Alternatively, problems on ToM tasks may be due to the increased saliency of self-related knowledge, which is often pitted against the information held by the other person in ToM tasks (e.g., in the Sally-Anne task, in which the observer has knowledge of where an object has been moved to, after a stooge has walked out of a room; cf. Wimmer & Perner, 1983). It is also unclear whether increased influences of self-related information in memory reflect a strategic emphasis under conditions of reduced memory capacity, or whether this reflects changes in self-related processing more generally.

We set out to address these questions in a study on the effects of aging on self-related processing and on the processing of other people, in a simple perceptual-matching task that has previously been shown to produce highly reliable biases favoring self-related stimuli (Sui, He, & Humphreys, 2012) that are stable across individuals over time (Humphreys & Sui, 2015; Stolte, Humphreys, Yankouskaya, & Sui, 2015). We had young (<30 years) and two groups of older adults (60–69 and 69+ years)Footnote 1 make associations between personal labels (You, Friend, and Stranger) and neutral geometric shapes (square, circle, and triangle), and they were told that the labels referred to themselves, their best friend, or a stranger who was not anyone they knew. Subsequently, participants either saw the original shape–label pairs (e.g., square–you, circle–friend) or saw the stimuli re-paired (e.g., square–friend, circle–you). The task was to decide whether the stimuli were in their original pairing or had been re-paired. Young participants show a large advantage for matching self-related pairs (square–you) over stimuli that they associate with their best friend (circle–friend), as well as an advantage for friend-related stimuli over items associated with a stranger (triangle–stranger; Sui et al., 2012). This self advantage increases when stimuli are perceptually degraded, suggesting enhanced perception for self-related stimuli. We asked whether this basic form of self bias in the perceptual-matching task is modulated by age.

Previous work has consistently shown that memory in young adults is better for self-related stimuli than for stimuli related to other people—the self-reference effect in memory (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Cunningham, Turk, Macdonald, & Macrae, 2008; Symons & Johnson, 1997). Recent research has shown that this self bias in memory is maintained in older people, though they have reduced memory capacity in general (e.g., Gutchess Kensinger, & Schacter, 2010; Gutchess, Kensinger, Yoon, & Schacter, 2007). Contrasting to past work on memory, in the present study we tested the effect of aging on the perceptual-matching task. One interesting aspect of the self-bias effect in perceptual matching is that it is related to an underlying neural circuitry (Sui, Rotshtein, & Humphreys, 2013). Notably, activation of ventral brain regions is associated with self processing and attention (respectively in the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex [VMPFC] and the posterior superior temporal sulcus) when self-related stimuli are presented, whereas enhanced activation of a dorsal frontoparietal network is associated with attentional control when participants respond to stimuli related to a stranger. These data indicate that attentional-control processes are recruited more strongly in the latter condition (see Sui & Humphreys, 2015a, for a discussion). Additionally, neuropsychological studies have shown that brain lesion over the VMPFC abolishes self biases in memory, whereas brain damage in the dorsal attentional-control network that spares the VMPFC generates abnormally large biases toward self-related stimuli, due to an exaggerated effect of strong attentional signals (Sui, Enock, Ralph, & Humphreys, 2015). Researchers have argued that self-bias effects are determined by the interaction between the ventral network through the VMPFC and the dorsal attentional-control network for control of behavior (Sui, 2016). From these findings, we may predict that self biases in perceptual matching may increase as people age because of a reduction in executive control, which should most strongly affect responses to stimuli associated with strangers.

On the other hand, work from the domain of social and clinical psychology has suggested that changes in cognition in older adults can reflect strategic changes in processing. For example, older adults appear to place more attentional weight on positive relative to negative information, leading to stronger positivity biases in memory than in younger participants (Mather & Carstensen, 2005). Older adults also show biases in social preference toward a smaller number of familiar social partners, partly because of strategic reflection on the lack of perceived time, as well as greater response to the positive emotional associations linked to such partners (Fung, Carstensen, & Lutz, 1999). These results offer alternative hypotheses about how older adults may vary in their perceptual matching for self, friend, and stranger-related stimuli. For example, if aging leads to an increased bias toward friend-related stimuli, due to the weighting of positive emotion and familiarity, it is possible that the friend advantage (relative to performance with stimuli related to a stranger) may increase as people age, whereas the self advantage (relative to the friend) may actually decrease. This would contradict an account based on the effects of decreasing executive function in older adults for the more difficult associations (friend and stranger).

In summary, we assessed the effects of aging on self- and other-related processing in the perceptual-matching task. We would predict an enhancing effect of self biases as people age, if reduced executive capacity affects responses to other-related stimuli in perceptual matching. Alternatively if strategic changes in processing among older adults modulate perceptual matching by increasing the weight on positive emotions and familiarity linked to close others, then we might expect an increased effect to their best friend with a decreased bias to the self over friends. To provide additional evidence on the latter possibility, we not only had participants carry out perceptual matching, but also asked them to rate the perceived personal distances between themselves, their best friend, and a (named) stranger. If aging biases processing toward a familiar friend, the judged personal distances from the self to the friend and from the friend to a stranger might vary across different age groups (the self distance from the friend reducing and the friend distance from the stranger increasing). Moreover, variations in perceived personal distance may predict the self and friend biases on perceptual matching. This would provide converging evidence for an effect of social coding, rather than executive functions per se, modulating cognition in older adults.

Method

Participants

We recruited 103 participants in total, including 36 in the young group (nine males, 27 females; ages 19–29 years, mean age 22.08 ± 2.60 years), 33 participants in the 60–69 group (16 males, 17 females; ages 61–69 years, mean age 65.58 ± 2.42 years), and 34 participants (15 males, 19 females; ages 70–86 years, mean age 75.59 ± 4.69 years) in the 69+ group. The sample size was determined from prior studies in order to get a reasonable effect size (e.g., Humphreys & Sui, 2015; Sui et al., 2012). All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment according to procedures approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Sciences Division of Oxford University.

Stimuli and tasks

Three geometric shapes (triangle, circle, and square) were assigned to three personal labels representing the participant, his or her named best friend (whom he or she was asked to name), and a stranger (he or she was asked to give a name that was familiar but not held by any personal acquaintance). The assignment of shapes to the different people was counterbalanced across participants. Before the experiment, participants were asked to name one of their gender-matched best friends and to select a gender-matched name as a stranger from a list of common names for people they did not know personally. The experiment then began with a shape–person association instruction presented on the screen. For example, participants were told that “the triangle represents your best friend, Mary, the square represents yourself, and the circle represents a stranger, Lucy.” The order of the three shape–label assignments during the instruction was counterbalanced across participants. The shape images were not presented at this stage. The instruction took about 1 min.Footnote 2 After the associative instruction, participants immediately performed a shape–label matching task to judge whether a shape–label pair matched. The same three shapes were used throughout all of the experimental trials. A shape occupying 3.5° × 3.5° of visual angle appeared above a white central fixation cross, 0.8° × 0.8° of visual angle. One of three personal labels (You, Friend, or Stranger, covering 1.76°/2.52° × 1.76° of visual angle) was displayed below the fixation cross. The task was to decide whether the shape–label pair was the same one initially shown or whether the shape and label had been re-paired. All stimuli were displayed on a gray background. The E-Prime software (version 2.0) was used to present the stimuli and record responses. The experiment was run on a PC with a 22-in. monitor (1,920 × 1,080 pixels) at 60 Hz.

Following the matching task, participants were required to indicate the personal distance between any two people by making two marks on a straight line on an A4 sheet (i.e., self and stranger, friend and stranger, self and friend), with the physical distance between the marks (in millimeters) serving as an index of the personal distance between the individuals (see Fig. 2b in the Results, as well as Sui & Humphreys, 2015b). The friend and the stranger referred to those whom participants had named before the experiment. There were ten trials per pairing; half the trials started with one person (e.g., self–friend), and the other half started with the other person (friend–self). To rule out individual variations across the age groups, personal distance (i.e., self–friend and friend–stranger) was also scaled by the distance between the participant and the stranger as relative scores (see Fig. 3 below). One participant in the 60–69 group and two participants in the 69+ group did not report personal distance.

Procedure

Participants completed the experiment individually in a quiet testing room. For the matching task, participants were instructed to associate one of three shapes with themselves, their best friend, or a stranger. No images of stimuli were displayed during the instruction stage (e.g., “circle = you,” “square = your best friend,” “triangle = a stranger”). After the instruction, participants had to judge whether a simultaneously presented shape-and-label pair matched (Fig. 1). Each trial started with a central fixation cross for 500 ms, followed by a shape–label pair at the center of the screen, for 100 ms for the young group and 500 ms for the older participants. Half of the pairings of the shape and label conformed to the instruction and were responded to as match trials; on the remaining trials, the shapes and labels were re-paired to form mismatch trials. For mismatch trials, a shape was paired with each of the other two possible labels (e.g., the self shape with either the friend or the stranger label). The next frame was a blank field lasting 1,000 ms for the young participants, or 3,000 ms for the old participants.Footnote 3 Participants were encouraged to make a response as quickly and accurately as possible within this interval by pressing one of the two keys on a keyboard with the index or middle finger of the right hand. The order of the response keys was counterbalanced across participants. A feedback message (Correct, Incorrect, or Too Slow!) was then given at the center of the screen for 500 ms. Participants were also informed of their overall accuracy at the end of each block. The experiment consisted of six blocks of 60 trials each, following 24 practice trials.Footnote 4 Thus, there were 60 trials for each match and mismatch condition.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of the stimuli and trial procedure in the matching task. A trial began with a 500-ms fixation displayed in the center of the screen, followed by a shape–label pairing presented for 100 ms for young participants or 500 ms for older individuals. The response deadlines were 1,000 ms for young participants or 3,000 ms for older adults. A 500-ms feedback presentation was then given to indicate whether a response was correct

Data analysis

Raw scores (Fig. 1)

For the behavioral measures, there was no trade-off between reaction times (RTs) and accuracy for any condition. The data on RTs and the d-prime results (computed from performance on match trials contrasted against mismatch trials with the same shape; see Sui et al., 2012) are reported separately. The data analyses for RTs were conducted separately for the match and mismatch trials, due to the different responses being made in these cases (e.g., Sui & Humphreys, 2014; Sun, Fuentes, Humphreys, & Sui, 2016). Repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted with Association (self, friend, or stranger) as a within-subjects factor and Age (young, 60–69, or 69+) as a between-subjects factor. For the measures of self-reported personal distance, we conducted repeated measures ANOVAs with Distance (self–friend, self–stranger, or friend–stranger) as a within-subjects factor and Age (young, 60–69, or 69+) as a between-subjects factor.

Bias scores

To assess changes in the self and friend biases over age, as well as to rule out individual variation across the three age groups, both the behavioral and self-reported data were normalized. We conducted ANOVAs using normalized bias scores with Bias (self vs. friend) as a within-subjects factor and Age (young, 60–69, or 69+) as a between-subjects factor. For RTs, the self bias was measured by the difference in performance for the self versus the friend condition, divided by the sum across the two conditions. The friend bias was indexed by the difference in performance for the friend condition against the stranger condition, divided by the sum across the two conditions. The self bias was indexed by the differential scores relative to the friend rather than the stranger condition so that we could compare the differences between the self and friend biases while controlling the familiarity effect. For d', the self and friend biases were indexed by the differential scores between the two conditions. For both RTs and d', a larger score indicated a larger bias, whereas smaller scores indicated a relatively a small bias. To normalize self-reported personal distance, we computed the self–friend distance scaled by the ratio of the participant–best friend distance to the self–stranger distance; the friend–stranger distance was measured by the ratio of the best friend–stranger distance to the self–stranger distance. In this case, a larger score indicated a greater relative personal distance between the two people, whereas a smaller score indicates a relatively shorter personal distance. There were similar patterns in RTs and in self-reported personal distance (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

(a, b) Performance in perceptual matching as a function of age (young, 60–69, or 69+) and association (self, friend, or stranger), shown for absolute reaction times (a) and d' (b). (c) Absolute personal distance measures as a function of age (young, 60–69, or 69+) and comparison (self–friend, self–stranger, or friend–stranger). Error bars represent one standard error

These bias scores were also used to calculate the relations between the behavioral measures and the self-reported personal distance scores. We conducted Spearman correlation analyses between the two types of measures. Given the ANOVA results indicating differences between the young and older participants, correlations were calculated for young and older participants separately.

For all critical comparisons, Holm–Bonferroni corrections at α = .05 were applied to all multiple comparisons (Holm, 1979). We report effect sizes using partial eta-squared (η p 2) for ANOVAs and Cohen’s dz for t tests (Lakens, 2013).

Results

Behavioral measures

Match and mismatch trials were analyzed separately, since they reflect separate decision criteria (see Sui et al., 2012).

RTs in match trials: Absolute RTs

The analysis of match trials revealed a significant main effect of association, F(2, 200) = 44.62, p < .001, η p 2 = .31, with faster responses to the self than to friend-associated (p = .001) and stranger-associated (p < .001) trials, and faster responses to the friend than to stranger-associated trials (p < .001). We also discovered a significant main effect of age, F(2, 100) = 22.38, p < .001, η p 2 = .31, reflecting faster responses in the young than in the 60–69 (p < .001) and 69+ (p < .001) groups, as well as faster responses in the 60–69 than in the 69+ group (p = .03). The main effects were qualified by a significant interaction between the association and age, F(4, 200) = 3.99, p < .005, η p 2 = .07 (Fig. 2a). The interaction was broken down for each of the three groups separately. The analysis in the young group showed a significant effect of association, F(2, 70) = 60.44, p < .001, η p 2 = .63, with faster responses to the self than to both friend-associated [t(35) = –8.62, p < .001, dz = 1.44] and stranger-associated [t(35) = –8.32, p < .001, dz = 1.37] trials, and faster responses to the friend than to stranger trials [t(35) = –2.79, p = .008, dz = 0.44]. We also found a significant effect of association in the 60–69 group, F(2, 64) = 20.32, p < .001, η p 2 = .39. Unlike in the young group, no difference in responses emerged between the self and friend trials [t(32) = –1.25, p = .22, dz = 0.22], but there were faster responses to both the self [t(32) = –6.92, p < .001, dz = 1.21] and friend [t(32) = –4.19, p = .001, dz = 0.73] trials than to the stranger trials. Likewise, a significant effect of association was apparent in the 69+ group, F(2, 66) = 11.99, p < .001, η p 2 = .27, reflecting faster responses to the self [t(33) = –3.88, p < .001, dz = 0.66] and friend [t(33) = –3.85, p = .001, dz = 0.66] trials than to the stranger trials, but we observed no difference in responses to the self and friend trials [t(33) = –1.85, p = .07, dz = 0.32].

Bias measures

Each of the biases was assessed across the three age groups separately using normalized bias scores, with Bias (self vs. friend; see the Method section) as a within-subjects factor and Age (young, 60–69, or 69+) as a between-subjects factor. The normalized scores here equated to differences in baseline RTs across the age groups. The analysis failed to show a significant main effect of either bias or age, F(2, 100) = 1.14, p = .29; F(2, 100) = 2.08, p = .13. However, we did observe a significant interaction between bias and age, F(2, 100) = 6.20, p = .003, η p 2 = .11. The between-subjects analysis failed to show significant differences across the age groups in the self-bias effect, F(2, 100) = 1.42, p = .25 (Fig. 3, upper left figure). However, there was a significant effect of age for the friend bias, F(2, 100) = 9.64, p < .001, η p 2 = .16. Both the 60–69 [t(67) = 4.70, p < .001, dz = 1.13] and 69+ [t(68) = 3.06, p = .003, dz = 0.73] groups showed larger friend biases than did the young group, but we found no difference between the 60–69 and 69+ groups [t(65) = 1.49, p = .14, dz = 0.36] (Fig. 3, upper right figure). These results indicated an enhanced friend bias with age.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Patterns of self (vs. friend) and friend (vs. stranger) biases in reaction times (RTs, top panel) and self-reported personal distance (bottom panel). The plots on the left side represent self biases in RTs (upper part) and in self-reported personal distance (lower part). The plots on the right side represent friend biases in RTs (upper part) and self-reported personal distance (lower part). Error bars represent one standard error of the normalized differential scores

To verify this result, we compared how many participants showed a larger self than friend bias within the young versus the old (60–69 and 69+) groups. Relative to the older participants (combining the 60–69 and 69+ age groups), young participants were more likely to demonstrate a larger self bias than a friend bias (83% of individuals in the young group vs. 30% of individuals in the old groups), χ 2 = 26.82, p < .001.

RTs on mismatch trials

The analysis of mismatch trials did not show either a significant main effect of association, F(2, 200) = 2.52, p = .08, η p 2 = .03, or an interaction between association and age, F(4, 200) = 0.85, p = .49, η p 2 = .02. However, a significant main effect of age, F(2, 200) = 21.23, p < .001, η p 2 = .30, reflected faster responses in the young than in either the 60–69 (p < .001) or the 69+ (p < .001) group. No differences emerged between the 60–69 and 69+ groups (p = .46).

d-prime

The analysis of d' showed a significant main effect of association, F(2, 200) = 56.20, p < .001, η p 2 = .36, with larger d' scores for the self condition than for the friend (p < .001) and stranger (p < .001) conditions, and a greater d' in the friend than in the stranger condition (p < .001; Fig. 2b). However, neither the main effect of age nor the interaction between association and age was significant, F(2, 100) = 2.06, p = .13, F(4, 200) = 1.46, p = .22.

In line with the RT data, we also conducted a chi-square analysis for the numbers of participants with a large or a small self bias relative to their friend bias. The analysis showed that the participants in the young group were more likely to have a large self bias (vs. friend bias; 81% of individuals in the young group) than were those in the old (60–69 and 69+) groups (55% of individuals in those groups), χ 2 = 6.53, p = .01.

Personal distance

Absolute distance

The analysis demonstrated a significant main effect of distance, F(2, 194) = 371.13, p < .001, η p 2 = .79; the rated distance was shorter for the self–friend comparison than for the self–stranger (p = .001) and friend–stranger (p < .001) comparisons, and the rated distance for friend–stranger was shorter than that for self–stranger (p = .046). We also observed a significant main effect of age, F(2, 97) = 9.49, p < .001, η p 2 = .16; young individuals reported larger personal distances than did the 60–69 (p < .001) and 69+ (p = .015) groups, but there was no difference between the 60–69 than 69+ groups (p = .57). The interaction between distance and age was marginal, F(4, 194) = 2.22,p = .068, η p 2 = .07 (Fig. 2c).

In line with the RT analysis, this interaction was broken down into the three age groups. The young group showed a significant effect of distance, F(2, 70) = 124.57,p < .001, η p 2 = .78, with a larger distance in the self–stranger than in the self–friend [t(35) = 13.32, p < .001, dz = 2.21] and friend–stranger [t(35) = 4.32, p < .001, dz = 0.25] comparisons, and the distance was larger for the friend–stranger than for the self–friend comparison [t(35) = 10.14, p < .001, dz = 2.56]. The effect of distance was also significant in the 60–69 group, F(2, 62) = 102.37, p < .001, η p 2 = .77. Unlike in the young group, no difference in distance was apparent between self–stranger and friend–stranger [t(31) = –0.33, p = .74, dz = 0.07], but the distances were both larger for the self–stranger [t(31) = 10.28, p < .001, dz = 3.04] and friend–stranger [t(31) = 10.27, p < .001, dz = 3.47] comparisons than for the self–friend comparison. Similarly, there was a significant effect of distance in the 69+ group, F(2, 62) = 175.24, p < .001, η p 2 = .85. The distances were comparable for the self–stranger and friend–stranger comparisons [t(31) = –1.31, p = .27, dz = 0.01], whereas there was a larger rated distance for the self–stranger [t(31) = 13.60, p < .001, dz = 2.21] and friend–stranger [t(31) = 14.05, p < .001, dz = 2.19] comparisons than for the self–friend comparison.

Bias scores

To assess the aging effect on perceived personal distance, we conducted ANOVAs using relative distance scores, normalized by the rated distance between the self and the stranger (to equate for possible age differences in the lengths of the scales used). The Bias in distance was a within-subjects factor (self–friend vs. friend–stranger; see the Method section), and Age was a between-subjects factor (young, 60–69, or 69+). We found a significant main effect of distance, F(2, 97) = 167.35, p < .001, η p 2 = .63, reflecting a smaller distance in the self–friend than in the friend–stranger comparison. The effect of age was also significant, F(2, 97) = 14.06, p < .001, η p 2 = .23, with larger relative perceived personal distances in both the 60–69 (p < .001) and 69+ (p = .006) groups than in the young group, but no difference between the 60–69 and 69+ groups (p = .15). We also observed a significant interaction between personal distance bias and age, F(2, 97) = 13.65, p < .001, η p 2 = .22.

For the self–friend distance (the self-bias effect), there was no significant effect of age, F(2, 97) = 1.84, p = .17 (Fig. 3, the lower left figure). In contrast, we did find a significant effect of age for the friend–stranger distance (the friend-bias effect), F(2, 97) = 13.86, p < .001, η p 2 = .22: Older participants in both the 60–69 [t(66) = 5.46, p < .001, dz = 1.33] and 69+ [t(66) = 3.85, p < .001, dz = 0.94] groups assigned a greater distance between their best friend and the stranger than did the young participants [we found no difference between the 60–69 and 69+ groups; t(62) = 1.620, p = .11, dz = 0.041] (Fig. 3, lower right figure). These results indicated an enhanced perceived friend–stranger distance, whereas the self–friend distance was maintained over age.

There were similar patterns in both RTs and self-reported personal distance (Fig. 3). Next, we conducted correlation analyses to assess the relations between these two types of measures.

Correlations between behavioral measures and personal distance

We conducted correlation analyses for young and older (60–69 and 69+) participants separately. We calculated correlations between the friend and self biases in normalized behavioral scores (for both friend–stranger and self–friend) and personal distance (friend–stranger/self–stranger and self–friend/self–stranger) (see the Method section). For d', Spearman correlation analyses in the young participants failed to show any indication of significant correlations between the behavioral biases and perceived personal distance, rs = –.01 and –.06, ps = .96 and .73, for the friend and self biases, respectively (Fig. 4, upper panel). In contrast, for the older group, we observed a significant positive correlation between the behavioral friend bias and perceived friend–stranger distance, r = .36, p = .003 (Fig. 4, lower left plot); older adults who tended to show a larger reported personal distance between friends and strangers also showed greater increases in perceptual sensitivity in matching friend- over stranger-associated shapes. However, no significant correlation was apparent in self biases, r = .05, p = .72 (Fig. 4, lower right panel). For RTs, no significant correlations appeared for either the young or the older groups, ps > .36

Fig. 4
figure 4

Spearman correlations between normalized differential scores in d' and self-reported personal distance. (a) For the young group, no significant correlations are apparent in either friend bias (upper left plot) or self bias (upper right plot). (b) For the older group, a significant positive correlation can be seen in friend bias (lower left plot), but not in self bias (lower right plot)

Discussion

The results showed that self-bias effects on perceptual matching were at least maintained across the age range (when bias effects were normalized for absolute RTs; but in terms of absolute RTs, self biases over familiar friends decreased with age). These data on perceptual matching were paralleled by the findings when participants rated the personal distance between themselves, their familiar friend, and a named stranger. The perceived distance between the self and the friend was maintained across the age groups (Fig. 3). This runs counter to the view that exacerbated executive demands on older participants reduce matching performance on other-related stimuli, and thus increase the self-bias effect (cf. Sui & Humphreys, 2015a), but these findings in perceptual matching are consistent with prior studies of the self-referential effect on memory (Gutchess et al., 2010; Gutchess et al., 2007), suggesting maintained effects of self-reference at different stages of processing more generally.

On the other hand, older participants showed larger friend biases than did younger participants (against a baseline of responses to strangers). This result in terms of the friend bias is consistent with the view that, relative to younger participants, older individuals tend to weigh familiar friends more in their perceptual judgments. It may also reflect an enhanced familiarity of their friends. Older participants are likely to have known their best friend for longer than younger participants, and the strength of the response to the friend may depend on the familiarity of the person. A further possibility (not mutually exclusive) is that older participants may show a more positive emotional response to their best friend partly because older individuals weight positive emotional value higher than do younger participants (Fung et al., 1999; Mather & Carstensen, 2005). A positive emotional response to the friend association may enhance the friend bias (see Stolte et al., 2015). Consistent with these results reflecting the social coding of friend relative to stranger stimuli, we found that the rated personal distance of the best friend from the stranger increased with age. Additionally, we observed a correlation between the friend bias (friend–stranger) in the d' measure in perceptual matching and the rated perceptual distance between the friend and the stranger: Individuals who rated the friend as being more distant from the stranger also showed a larger bias in their sensitivity when matching friend over stranger stimuli in perceptual matching. Aron and colleagues proposed that individuals use close relationships to achieve self-expansion through the inclusion of others in the self (Aron, Aron, & Norman, 2001; Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). The present results are in line with the view that older participants favor their best friend more than do young adults. The increased inclusion of the friend in the self in older adults, then, may lead to a larger rated personal distance between the friend and the stranger, and a larger bias toward the friend over stranger stimuli in the matching task, but this was contradicted by the maintained self bias (vs. the friend) in perceptual matching, although the bias toward the self over friends did tend to decrease with age (see Fig. 3, upper left plot). We found no other direct relationships between perceived personal distance and biases in perceptual matching. We conclude that both the rating of personal distance and the sensitivity in perceptual matching are modulated by the social coding of the friend relative to the stranger, which changes as people age. In contrast, biases toward the self in perceptual matching do not increase with age.

A somewhat different account of the changes in self- and friend-related processing in older people is based on interference effects.Footnote 5 Because it has been shown that older adults are more susceptible to the effects of proactive interference (Bowles & Salthouse, 2003; Emery, Hale, & Myerson, 2008), their maintained self bias and enhanced friend bias may stem from an increased proactive interference across trials in older people. For example, built-up interference may be stronger from self- and friend-related trials (trial n – 1) to stranger-related trials (trial n) than is the interference from stranger-related trials (trial n – 1) to self- and friend-related trials (trial n). However, the design of the present study did not allow for such analyses. These ideas may be tested in the future by formal analyses of carryover effects.

Though there are reductions in social-cognitive capacities (e.g., seeing other people’s perspectives in ToM tasks) as well as in cognitive functions (e.g., memory and executive control functions) in older people, the present results confirm that the basic form of self bias in perceptual matching is preserved as people age. This is consistent with evidence from previous work in the memory domain (e.g., recall, recognition, and source memory) showing a maintained self advantage in memory among older participants (Gutchess et al., 2010; Gutchess et al., 2007; Hamami, Serbun, & Gutchess, 2011; Mueller, Wonderlich, & Dugan, 1986; Rosa & Gutchess, 2011; Yang, Truong, Fuss, & Bislimovic, 2012). Importantly, we also found an enhancing effect of the friend bias in perceptual matching, which correlated with the perceived personal distance between friends and strangers. These results have broad implications for understanding social behavior in older people. For example, they can help explain why older adults become more selective toward familiar friends when asked to make social judgments (Fredrickson & Carstensen, 1990), since cognitive biases toward familiar friends would be reinforced by enhanced perception and attention to these individuals and to the information associated with them.