Seminal studies conducted by Pavlov (1927) and subsequent research by other investigators has identified the rules by which paired events become associated. In the simplest case, an animal may learn that one event (e.g., cue X) signals the pending occurrence of a second event (e.g., Outcome 1 [O1]). However, life is not always so simple, because in some cases an animal later experiences the cue that had been signaling Outcome 1 now predicting another event (e.g., Outcome 2 [O2]). Within the associative literature, many studies have examined situations in which a cue has been sequentially paired with more than one outcome, a procedure that often gives rise to what is called associative outcome interference. Specifically, associative outcome interference refers to the decrease in the cue’s potential on a posttraining test trial to elicit behavior anticipatory of O1 (i.e., retroactive interference) or O2 (i.e., proactive interference) as a result of the pairing with the nontarget outcome. This seemingly reflects the animal’s possessing conflicting information. Hence, on a test with the cue, the animal must decide whether to respond on the basis of the first or second training experience or some mixture of the two. Given the high frequency with which such situations arise in real life, it is important that we understand the factors that determine which of the two types of training will be expressed at test.

Among the most widely studied types of associative interference situations are extinction (i.e., pairings of the cue with a specific outcome during Phase 1 and unpaired presentations of the target cue during Phase 2) and latent inhibition (i.e., unpaired presentations of the target cue during Phase 1 and pairings of the cue with a specific outcome during Phase 2). As a result of pressure toward translational research, extinction has been by far the most widely studied associative interference situation because it is viewed as modeling exposure therapy for anxiety disorders and drug abuse, and recovery from extinction has been viewed as modeling relapse from exposure therapy (for a review, see Laborda, McConnell, & Miller, 2011).

Although a large number of studies of associative interference have been published, almost all of them have focused exclusively on what happens when one or another specific type of training occurs first (i.e., during Phase 1) and a different specific type of training occurs second (i.e., during Phase 2), rather than seek general principles that might apply independent of the specific nature of the training in Phase 1 and Phase 2 (but see Bouton, 1993). Here too we of course used specific types of training in Phase 1 and Phase 2, but we targeted types of training that have rarely been examined with respect to associative interference. The types of training examined here in and of themselves are of relatively little interest in our opinion. Rather, we examined them as part of a larger plan to look at a wide variety of different types of training in each phase of associative interference situations to collectively determine whether general rules obtain that are independent of the specific types of training administered in each phase.

In search of more general principles that transcend specific associative interference situations, we have been examining diverse interference situations in order to identify commonalities (and differences) across these situations as a function of (a) the specific nature of Phase 1 training and Phase 2 training (e.g., excitatory conditioning, Pavlovian inhibition training, and simple exposure to the cue, as in extinction and the latent inhibition treatment), (b) whether the target memory at test is what was trained in Phase 1 (i.e., retroactive interference) or in Phase 2 (i.e., proactive interference), and (c) the nature of the ambiguity between training in Phases 1 and 2. Ambiguity between the two training phases can arise from a number of differences such as the nature of the two outcomes or the temporal relationships between the cue and the outcomes (e.g., Escobar & Miller, 2003; Molet, Urcelay, Miguez, & Miller, 2010). The present experiments are a small contribution to the larger project of determining the general rules of associative outcome interference across diverse situations, assuming that there are any. Here we examined the test-context specificity of proactive interference, all other things being equal, in the specific case of nonreinforced presentations of the target cue during Phase 1 (i.e., a latent inhibition treatment) serving as the interfering treatment and Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training during Phase 2 serving as the target training that is potentially subject to interference.

In conventional proactive outcome interference situations in which “cue–no outcome” trials provided by a latent inhibition treatment or conditioned inhibition training in Phase 1 constitute the interfering memory, the target association assessed at test is excitatory (i.e., a CS–outcome association, with the outcome usually being an unconditioned stimulus [US]). However, preexposing a cue, as in a latent inhibition treatment, also results in a decrement in expression (or acquisition) of the second-learned association when the second-learned association is inhibitory (i.e., conditioned inhibition training rather than conditioned excitation training; e.g., Friedman, Blaisdell, Escobar, & Miller, 1998; Reiss & Wagner, 1972; Rescorla, 1971). This demonstrates that a form of outcome interference can also occur when the first-learned experience is devoid of an expectancy of any US (such as in a latent inhibition treatment) and the second-learned experience involves a violated expectancy of a specific US (such as in Pavlovian inhibition training). Both latent inhibitors and conditioned inhibitors are retarded in acquiring behavioral control during subsequent excitation training. However, they differ in that a latent inhibitor does not reduce responding to an excitatory CS (a transfer excitor) when it is presented in compound with the excitatory CS any more than does an unfamiliar stimulus, whereas a conditioned inhibitor does (e.g., Lorden, Rickert, & Berry, 1983; Reiss & Wagner, 1972). This observation indicates that a different mechanism underlies conditioned inhibition than the mechanism underlying latent inhibition and that summation tests are a sensitive means of differentiating between information acquired in the two treatments.

In the present research, we examined the nature of the dependency on the test context of interference with Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training (A–US/AX–) by a prior latent inhibition treatment (X–). That is, we were concerned with whether test context influenced this specific type of outcome interference when all other factors are held constant. Prior research in a number of interference situations, although not with a latent inhibition treatment followed by conditioned inhibition training, has generally found that the relative similarity of the test context to each of the training contexts is a critical factor in determining the impact each of the two training experiences will have on responding at test (e.g., Bouton, 1993; Miller & Laborda, 2011). The most widely studied case of context shift effects influencing outcome interference is renewal following extinction (for a review, see Westbrook & Bouton, 2010). In renewal, partial recovery of an extinguished response occurs when testing takes place outside the extinction context (e.g., Bouton & Bolles, 1979; Bouton & King, 1983). Data from outcome interference preparations other than extinction also speak to the dependency of outcome interference on the test context. For instance, a context shift between a latent inhibition treatment in Phase 1 and Pavlovian excitatory conditioning in Phase 2 results in less interference with expression of the CS–US association (i.e., stronger responding) when the CS is tested in the context in which excitation was trained, as compared to testing of the CS in the context of the latent inhibition treatment (i.e., ABA renewal vs. an ABB control; Bailey & Westbrook, 2008; Gray et al. 2001; Hall & Channell, 1985; Kaplan & Lubow, 2001; Lovibond, Preston, & Mackintosh, 1984; Nakajima, Takahashi, & Blaisdell, 2006; Nelson & Sanjuan, 2006; Rosas & Bouton, 1997; Rudy, 1994; Schiller & Weiner, 2005; Zalstein-Orda & Lubow, 1995) or in a novel context (Bouton & Swartzentruber, 1989; Maes, 2002; Wheeler, Chang, & Miller, 2003; Yap & Richardson, 2005). Moreover, when a latent inhibition treatment is administered in Context A and excitatory conditioning is administered in Context B, a greater decrement in excitatory responding has been observed in Context C relative to Context B than is seen with the same context switch without the initial latent inhibition treatment. This could be viewed as indicating some degree of transfer of latent inhibition beyond the context used for latent inhibition treatment (Swartzentruber & Bouton, 1992). Similarly, Lovibond et al. (1984), Exp. 2) demonstrated that a switch out of the context used for latent inhibition treatment severely reduces, but does not always completely eliminate, the latent inhibition effect.

In the present Experiment 2, we examined the role of test context in an inhibitory analogue of the studies of latent inhibition followed by excitatory conditioning that are described above. The analogy arises from Pavlovian inhibition training being administered in Phase 2 in place of excitatory conditioning of the target cue. In Experiment 2, in which the latent inhibition treatment was administered in Context A during Phase 1 and conditioned inhibition training was administered in Context B during Phase 2, we expected to see behavior reflecting the latent inhibition treatment when the target cue X was tested in Context A (i.e., an ABA procedure) and behavior indicative of the conditioned inhibition treatment when X was tested in Context B (i.e., an ABB procedure). This would parallel what is observed when a latent inhibition treatment in one context is followed by excitatory conditioning in a different context. In Experiment 1, a latent inhibition treatment and subsequent inhibitory conditioning were both administered in the same context (A), and the target cue was tested in either Context A (i.e., an AAA procedure) or a context in which the target cue had not been trained (B; i.e., an AAB procedure). Here the results were less certain, because prior research with a latent inhibition treatment and conditioned excitation training had shown the [first-learned] latent inhibition treatment to be context specific (e.g., Hall & Channell, 1985; Swartzentruber & Bouton, 1992), but also had shown second-learned Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training, following conditioned excitation training, to be context specific (e.g., Nelson, 2002; Sissons & Miller, 2009; Westbrook, Jones, Bailey, & Harris, 2000). If second-learned conditioned inhibition is context specific in this situation (i.e., with the latent inhibition treatment in Phase 1), as has been observed when excitatory conditioning is trained in Phase 1, little conditioned inhibition should be observed when the testing occurs outside the common context of latent inhibition and conditioned inhibition training. However, without the initial latent inhibition treatment (which makes a subsequent conditioned inhibition training second learned), conditioned inhibition training in Context A should readily transfer to Context B.

Most accounts of associative interference were developed on the basis of data from situations in which at least one of the two contradictory associations is excitatory (e.g., Bouton, 1994; Laborda & Miller, 2012, Miller & Laborda, 2011). Thus, the dependency of associative interference on the test context when a latent inhibition treatment precedes conditioned inhibition training may not adhere to the rules that have been identified for situations in which one phase of training involves excitatory conditioning of the target cue (i.e., CS–US pairings). Clearly, proactive interference with conditioned inhibition (acquisition or expression) sometimes results from a prior latent inhibition treatment (e.g., Friedman et al. 1998), but the degree of context specificity of this interference is currently unclear.

Only one previous article has been directly relevant to the issues raised here, and it allows for no firm conclusions. Nakajima et al. (2006, Exp. 2) failed to observe any context specificity of interference with conditioned inhibition by latent inhibition. Using rats in a food magazine approach task, Nakajima et al. first preexposed rats to a buzzer in either Context A or B (or no preexposure for a control group). Then all subjects received reinforced trials of a light in Context A, followed by more reinforced presentations of the light interspersed with nonreinforced trials of the light–buzzer compound also in Context A. As a result of this light–US / light + buzzer–no US procedure, the buzzer presumably became a conditioned inhibitor. The authors compared the rates of acquisition of conditioned inhibition across groups and found that preexposure to the buzzer retarded development of conditioned inhibition, relative to no preexposure. But they observed no difference in retardation as a function of whether preexposure to the buzzer had occurred in Context A or B.

The lack of context specificity of latent inhibition’s effect on conditioned inhibition observed in Nakajima et al.’s (2006) study, however, should be regarded with caution. First, unexpected differences across groups were found during the light–food training phase, though there was convergence by the end of light–food training. Nevertheless, that difference could have interacted later with any effect that latent inhibition might have had on conditioned inhibition. Second, their assessment of conditioned inhibition consisted of comparing responding to the light alone with responding to the light–buzzer compound, which constitutes an unusual test for conditioned inhibition. Notably, there was no summation test for conditioned inhibition using a transfer conditioned excitor compounded with the putative conditioned inhibitor and control condition in which the transfer excitor was compounded with a neutral cue to control for external inhibition. Such a summation test is widely recognized as being necessary to certify that a stimulus is a conditioned inhibitor (Rescorla, 1969). Third, in their Experiment 1, the simple context dependency of the latent inhibition treatment’s effect on excitatory conditioning was small, only observed with liberal statistical analyses, which suggests that the procedures that they used were not particularly sensitive to context shift effects in general. For these reasons, we believe that further study is warranted. Furthermore, the intent of the present experiments differed somewhat from that of Nakajima’s Experiment 2. Nakajima et al. varied whether conditioned inhibition training occurred in the same or in a different context from that of the prior latent inhibition treatment, and then they assessed the rate of development of conditioned inhibition, whereas within each of our experiments we held constant the context of the latent inhibition treatment and the context of conditioned inhibition training, and then we varied the test context.

To maintain comparability with many other studies of associative interference in which two phases of training have been followed by a test, our focal interest was in the context specificity of the expression of (proactive outcome) interference in a test phase following the completion of the latent inhibition treatment in Phase 1 and the completion of conditioned inhibition training in Phase 2. Toward this end, we assessed interference in various contexts using (negative) summation tests for conditioned inhibition that were administered following completion of conditioned inhibition training. This contrasts with Nakajima et al. (2006), who administered their test during Phase 2, which presumably assessed differences in the rates of acquisition of stimulus control during Phase 2 (i.e., before conditioned inhibition training was complete). Importantly, in both of the following experiments, all contexts were equated for total exposure and for nonassociative experience in each phase of the study, a feature that was lacking in Nakajima et al.’s Experiment 2, which otherwise was in several respects conceptually similar to the present Experiment 2. Here, “equating nonassociative experience of the contexts” means administering cue and cue–outcome presentations, but with irrelevant cues substituted for the experimental cues during phases in which the target cue was not trained in a given context, thereby matching the contexts with respect to their associative histories with the outcome. Centrally, in both of the present experiments we assessed control by the test context of interference with conditioned inhibition by prior latent inhibition treatment; that is, in a situation in which neither type of training (i.e., latent inhibition or conditioned inhibition) involved pairings of the target cue with the outcome. This is what makes the present research novel, save for Nakajima et al.’s Experiment 2, which we view as being suggestive but equivocal. In each of our two studies, we gave all four critical groups Phase 1 treatment in the same context and Phase 2 treatment in the same context. Then we varied where the testing occurred. In this way we were able to examine whether test location per se influenced the expression of latent inhibition effects on conditioned inhibition, with all other factors held constant. Because we did so, this study served as a (small) part of a larger project concerned with the similarities and differences in the rules of associative interference across diverse interference situations. Additionally, we were interested in whether the expression of associative interference can be modified by the specific test context, which speaks to the question of whether this type of interference reflects an absence of the target information or a failure to express the target information.

Experiment 1

In Experiment 1 (see Table 1), we assessed the influence of test context on latent inhibition’s interference with Pavlovian conditioned inhibition by administering a summation test with a transfer excitor in the context that was used for both the latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training (A) as well as in a “nontraining” context (B); thus, we examined the effects of an AAB context shift relative to an AAA control. By “nontraining” context, we refer to a context in which events occurred equivalent to those events that occurred during target training, but with irrelevant cues rather than the critical experimental cues. This assured that the nontraining context (B) was matched to the context used for target training (A) with respect to context–US associations. Only a summation test was used because latent inhibition treatments as well as conditioned inhibition training produce retardation of development of excitatory control of behavior by the target cue. Hence, a retardation test, in which the development of excitatory behavioral control by the target cue was assessed, would have been confounded by differences across groups in the presence or absence of the latent inhibition treatment. The procedure for the conditioned inhibition training was borrowed almost exactly from Polack, Laborda, and Miller (2014), who demonstrated that this specific preparation produced a conditioned inhibitor that passed both summation and retardation tests for conditioned inhibition. Our use of these previously established parameters obviated the need for a demonstration that Phase 2 of training in the present research was adequate to produce conditioned inhibition.

Table 1 Design summary of Experiment 1

On the basis of the well-known context specificity of latent inhibition when followed with excitatory training, one might expect latent inhibition to also be context specific when followed by conditioned inhibition training, which would be manifest as reduced conditioned inhibition in the context of the latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training (A), but not in the context in which the target cue was not trained (B). However, when conditioned inhibition learning is the second-learned information concerning the target cue (X), the context specificity of second-learned information (Bouton, 1993) might be expected to result in weak conditioned inhibition when testing occurs in Context B (i.e., outside the context in which conditioned inhibition training occurred). This would be consistent with the previously reported context specificity of second-learned conditioned inhibition after conditioned excitation training during Phase 1 (Nelson 2002, Exp. 2; see also Sissons & Miller, 2009). The critical question in Experiment 1 was the degree of interference with the effects of conditioned inhibition training that would be observed at test in Context B with (i.e., Group AAB-X) and without (i.e., Group AAB-Con) the prior latent inhibition treatment (i.e., X–). A second cue (Y) was subjected to the latent inhibition treatment in Context B during Phase 1 and conditioned inhibition training in Context B during Phase 2, in order to equate the amounts of exposure and provide equivalent nonassociative experience and nontarget cue–context pairings in the two contexts (i.e., to equate the two contexts in all respects except for the presentations of the target CS). In Phase 3, a transfer excitor (R) for the subsequent summation test with X was trained in both Contexts A and B. Finally, XR summation tests were conducted in Contexts A and B. Two additional groups, AAA-X and AAA-Con, were treated identically to Groups AAB-X and AAB-Con, respectively, except that they were tested in Context A. Finally, a fifth group, Sum-Con, was included to assess the baseline behavior at test.

Method

Subjects

The subjects were 60 male, experimentally naive Sprague-Dawley descended rats obtained from Harlan Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN). Their body weight range was 163–211 g at the beginning of the experiment. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of five groups (ns = 12) and individually housed in standard plastic cages in a vivarium maintained on a 16/8-hr light/dark cycle. Experimental manipulations occurred near the middle portion of the light phase. The animals received free access to Purina Lab Chow pellets, whereas water availability in the home cage was limited to 30 min per day following a progressive deprivation schedule initiated 4 days prior to the start of the study. On experimental days, water in the home cage was provided 1–4 h after the end of each day’s experimental sessions. From the time of arrival in the laboratory until the start of the study, all animals were handled for 30 s, three times per week.

Apparatus

Twenty-four chambers of two distinct types, R and V (12 of each), were used. Each chamber was housed in a separate light- and sound-attenuating environmental isolation chest.

Chamber R was rectangular, measuring 24.0 × 9.0 × 12.5 cm (l × w × h). The walls and ceiling of Chamber R were clear Plexiglas, and the floor comprised stainless steel cylindrical rods measuring 0.5 cm in diameter, spaced 1.3 cm apart (center to center). The rods were connected by NE-2 bulbs, which permitted the delivery a 0.6-mA, 0.5-s constant-current footshock provided by a high voltage AC circuit in series with a 1.0-MΩ resistor. Chamber V was 27 cm long, 29.5 cm high, 21.5 cm wide at the top, and 5.5 cm wide at the bottom. The floor was composed of two 27-cm-long plates, 2 cm wide, with a 1.5-cm gap between the two plates. The ceiling was clear Plexiglas, the front and back walls were black Plexiglas, and the sidewalls and floor were stainless steel. A 0.6-mA, 0.5-s constant-current footshock, produced by a high voltage AC circuit in series with a 1.0-MΩ resistor, could be delivered through the metal walls and floor of the chamber.

Both types of chambers could be equipped with a water-filled lick tube that extended 1 cm into a 5-cm-deep cylindrical niche, which was 4.5 cm in diameter, left–right centered, with its bottom 1.75 cm above the floor of the apparatus. In all chambers a photobeam detector was positioned 1 cm in front of the lick tube that was broken whenever the subject licked the tube. A flashing light (0.25 s on / 0.25 s off) served as a visual cue. The light was produced by either a 25-W bulb (Chamber R) or a 100-W bulb (Chamber V) nominal at 120 VAC, but driven at 80 VAC. The bulbs were mounted on an inside wall of the environmental chest, approximately 30 cm from the center of the experimental chamber. The light intensities inside the animal chambers produced by the flashing light in the two chambers were approximately equal due to the difference in opacities of the walls. A 6S6 bulb (30 V, 6 W) mounted on the interior ceiling of the isolation chest in a corner served as the house light. In the R chambers this bulb was 2/3 blackened, whereas in V chambers the bulb was not masked, which resulted in approximately equivalent illumination in the two chambers due to the difference in opacities of the walls. The house light was normally on; turning it off served as a cue. Three 45-Ω speakers on the interior side and back walls of the isolation chests could deliver a click train (6 Hz), complex tone (500 and 520 Hz presented simultaneously), and white noise, each 8 dB above background. Ventilation fans in each enclosure provided a constant 76-dB background noise. All auditory measurements were on the C-scale. The clicks, tone, and white noise served as cues X, Y, and Z (or N), perfectly counterbalanced within groups. The house light (going off, as it was normally on) and the flashing light, perfectly counterbalanced within groups, served as training excitors P and Q during conditioned inhibition training of X and Y, respectively. Additionally, a SonAlert that could provide a 1900-Hz tone 8 dB above background was mounted on the ceiling of each environmental chest. The SonAlert served as the transfer excitor, R, on the summation test for conditioned inhibition. All audiovisual stimuli were 30 s in duration. The 0.5-s footshock served as the US.

For half of the subjects, Chamber R was Context A and Chamber V was Context B; this was reversed for the other half of the subjects. Chamber assignation was counterbalanced with respect to stimulus assignation using an incomplete Latin square. In each phase of the experiment, all subjects received equal exposure to Contexts A and B. For half of the animals in each group, treatment in Context A occurred on odd numbered days and treatment in Context B occurred on even numbered days; this was reversed for the other half of the animals. Subject assignation to treatment in Context A on odd versus even numbered days was counterbalanced using an incomplete Latin square with respect to cue designation.

Procedure (see Table 1)

Acclimation

On Days 1 and 2, all subjects were given 30 min of daily exposure to both Contexts A and B. This acclimation was intended to reduce the novelty of the contexts and train the rats to drink from the lick tube. The order of context exposure was counterbalanced within groups and reversed between Days 1 and 2. The order of context exposure was perfectly counterbalanced with respect to context, but was counterbalanced using an incomplete Latin square with respect to cue assignment. Sessions within a day were separated by approximately 1 h. Following Day 2, the lick tubes were removed until Reacclimation.

Phase 1

On Days 3, 5, 7, 9, and 11 (or Days 4, 6, 8, 10, and 12), each rat in Groups AAA-X, AAB-X, and Sum-Con received 60 daily presentations of target cue X during an 120.5-min session in Context A with a mean intertrial interval (ITI) of 90 s (range: 60–120 s), CS onset to CS onset, whereas the subjects in Groups AAA-Con and AAB-Con received similar treatment with nontarget cue Z. Additionally, all subjects received identical treatments on Days 4, 6, 8, 10, and 12 (or Days 3, 5, 7, 9, and 11) in Context B, except that cue Y was presented instead of cues X and Z. As we previously described, the actual order of the X (or Z) and Y sessions across days was counterbalanced within groups.

Phase 2

On Days 13, 15, and 17 (or Days 14, 16, and 18), all subjects experienced an 81-min session in Context A during which seven reinforced P and 21 nonreinforced PX trials occurred. On Days 14, 16, and 18 (or Days 13, 15, and 17), all subjects experienced an 81-min session in Context B during which seven reinforced Q and 21 nonreinforced QY trials occurred. On reinforced trials, the US was presented during the last 0.5 s of the 30-s cue presentation. Each session had an average of ITI of 143 s (range: 93–193) from cue onset to cue onset. The actual order of P/PX and Q/QY sessions was counterbalanced within groups as previously described.

Phase 3

On Day 19 (or Day 20), all subjects experienced 60-min sessions in Context A, during which one reinforced presentation of R occurred. On Day 20 (or Day 19), all subjects experienced 60-min sessions in Context B, during which one reinforced presentation of R occurred. The US was presented during the last 0.5 s of the 30-s presentation of R. On Day 19, the trial started 20 min into the session, whereas on Day 20 the trial started 40 min into the session. The order of training contexts was counterbalanced within groups.

Reacclimation

Reacclimation to drinking occurred in both Contexts A and B on both Days 21 and 22 with the lick tubes present. These sessions lasted 60 min per context, with about 120 min between sessions. The order of context exposure was counterbalanced within groups. These sessions were intended to restore baseline licking that might have been disrupted by the prior footshocks.

Test

On Day 23, the subjects in Conditions AAA and AAB were given a summation test during which the compound XR was presented within an 11-min session. The subjects in Group Sum-Con received presentations of the compound NR instead of XR, in order to assess behavioral control by the transfer excitor (R) in the absence of a conditioned inhibitor. Cue N was intended to control for external inhibition by X of behavioral control by R, as well as for the stimulus generalization decrement upon going from training on R alone to testing on XR (due to any possible configuring of R with its companion stimulus at test). In the AAB condition, subjects were tested in Context B. In the AAA condition, they were tested in Context A. Half of the subjects in Group Sum-Con were tested in Context A, and the other half in Context B. There was no basis to expect the data from these two subgroups to differ, because cue N had never been presented in either context, cue R had the same history in both contexts, and all other cues (X, Y, P, and Q) had been fully counterbalanced; hence, the data from these two subgroups were pooled. The test compound was presented to each rat when it had finished drinking for five cumulative seconds, and the time for the subject to drink an additional five cumulative seconds was then recorded. Thus, all rats were drinking at CS onset. Each animal received 10 min of exposure to the test compound before being removed from the chamber. Subjects taking more than 60 s to complete 5 s of baseline drinking (i.e., before CS onset) were eliminated from the study, because such behavior reflected an atypical reluctance to drink.

Results and discussion

The results of Experiment 1 are depicted in Fig. 1. One subject (from Group AAA-X) was eliminated according to the elimination criterion. A second subject (from Group AAB-Con) was eliminated due to an experimenter error.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Results of Experiment 1: Mean times to complete five cumulative seconds of drinking in the presence of the target test compound. Therefore, 0.7 log s is the lowest possible score. Higher scores represent stronger conditioned suppression. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. See the text and Table 1 for details

A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) conducted on baseline drinking (i.e., time to complete five cumulative seconds of drinking prior to CS onset) did not detect differences among the groups, F(4, 53) = 1.39, p = .24. A similar ANOVA performed on the time to drink five cumulative seconds in the presence of the CS did reveal differences among the groups, F(4, 53) = 10.91, MSE = 0.149, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.83 (95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.51, 1.14).

Planned comparisons using the error term from the ANOVA were conducted to identify the sources of these differences and to test specific hypotheses. As expected, greater conditioned suppression was observed in Group Sum-Con than in Groups AAA-Con and AAB-Con, F(1, 53) = 29.72, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.71 (95% CI = 0.42, 1.00), and F(1, 53) = 20.04, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.58 (95% CI = 0.30, 0.86), respectively. This is consistent with X acting as an effective conditioned inhibitor in either test context (given no latent inhibition treatment of X), whereas N never underwent conditioned inhibition training. That is, strong conditioned suppression was expected in Group Sum-Con because the transfer excitor (R) was tested in compound with a neutral cue (N), which should not have impacted conditioned suppression to R (beyond producing some external inhibition and stimulus generalization decrement that should also have been produced by X in Groups AAA-Con and AAB-Con). Group Sum-Con did not reliably differ from Groups AAA-X and AAB-X, F(1, 53) = 3.71, p = .059, and F(1, 53) = 0.75, p = .38, respectively. Strong conditioned suppression was observed in Group AAA-X relative to Group AAA-Con, F(1, 53) = 11.58, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.44 (95% CI = 0.31, 0.87). This essentially replicates Friedman et al. (1998) in showing that, when all treatment and testing occurs in a single context, the latent inhibition treatment interferes with the subsequent acquisition or expression of conditioned inhibition. Clearly, the inhibitory potential of X in Group AAA-X was attenuated relative to Group AAA-Con, due to interference as a result of the latent inhibition treatment of (i.e., nonreinforced exposure to) X in Phase 1. Similarly, the inhibitory potential of X in Group AAB-X was attenuated relative to Group AAB-Con, F(1, 53) = 13.16, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.47 (95% CI = 0.30, 0.86). This indicates that latent inhibition of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition transferred to Context B in an AAB paradigm, which was the central question being addressed in this experiment. The following statistics, excluding Group Sum-Con, further support this conclusion.

A 2 (Test Context: A or B) × 2 (Treatment: X or Con) ANOVA detected no interaction, p = .87. An effect of treatment was detected, representing more suppression to XR when X had been preexposed, F(1, 42) = 25.27, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.74 (95% CI = 0.41, 1.07), which indicates that the latent inhibition treatment disrupted negative summation by X during the test. The effect of test context was not significant, p = .17. That is, behavioral control by the XR compound in Condition X did not reliably differ, regardless of whether testing occurred in the treatment context (A) or outside of it (B).

In summary, when conditioned inhibition was the first-learned relationship concerning X (i.e., no preexposure to cue X during Phase 1; Groups AAA-Con and AAB-Con), behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition was observed not only in the training context but in a context in which the target cue had not been trained. However, when conditioned inhibition was learned second (i.e., following preexposure to X; Groups AAA-X and AAB-X), it was poorly acquired (or at least not expressed), due to the prior latent inhibition treatment when conditioned inhibition was assessed in a context different from that of treatment, as well as in the context in which latent inhibition and conditioned inhibition training had occurred. Importantly, the present results could not be due to differences in exposure to or nonassociative experiences in the two test contexts, because these were equated across the test contexts.

Although latent inhibition of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition transferred to Context B in an AAB paradigm, the absence of evidence of conditioned inhibition in Context B could have been due to either or both (a) the latent inhibition treatment of Group AAB-X in Phase 1 making the conditioned inhibition training in Phase 2 a second-learned association concerning cue X that did not transfer to Context B or (b) context-nonspecific interference with Phase 2 conditioned inhibition training by the Phase 1 latent inhibition treatment that transferred to Context B. One might argue that the relative lack of conditioned inhibition in Group AAB-X was not likely due to the effect of the latent inhibition treatment transferring into Context B because much prior research has demonstrated that latent inhibition is relatively context specific. For example, Hall and Minor (1984) found latent inhibition to produce a decrement in behavioral control following excitatory fear conditioning when the latent inhibition treatment and fear conditioning were both conducted in the same context relative to different contexts being used for the latent inhibition treatment and fear conditioning (for an analogous result with appetitive conditioning, see Channell & Hall, 1983). However, all of this prior research was conducted with excitatory training in Phase 2, and consequently might not apply to the present experiment in which inhibitory training was administered during Phase 2. Thus, there is no clear basis at present to differentiate between these two accounts of why latent inhibition treatment interfered with behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition training in Group AAB-X. However, the goal of Experiment 1 was to determine whether, after the Phase 1 latent inhibition treatment and Phase 2 Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training both in Context A, interference with behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition would be less pronounced in Context B than in Context A. We did not observe this.

Although the present data do not indicate why conditioned inhibition was not evident in Context B, empirically we see that when the latent inhibition treatment and subsequent conditioned inhibition training occurred in the same context, negative summation by the target inhibitor was attenuated in both the training context and a context in which the target cue had not been trained as an inhibitor. That is, the latent inhibition treatment resulted in similarly high levels of proactive interference with conditioned inhibition, regardless of whether testing occurred in the treatment context or in a context in which the target cue had not been trained. This conceptually replicates the findings of Friedman et al. (1998) in a situation with an aversive US, which is informative, given the previously described concerns with Nakajima et al.’s design and data.

Experiment 2

As in Experiment 1, in Experiment 2 we examined interference with Pavlovian conditioned inhibition in Phase 2 by the latent inhibition treatment administered in Phase 1 as a function of the context in which testing occurred. However, in Experiment 2 an ABA (vs. ABB) design was used, rather than an AAB (vs. AAA) design (see Table 2). Alternatively stated, here we assessed how test context influenced latent inhibition’s interference with conditioned inhibition, now with latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training administered in distinctively different contexts, by administering a summation test in each of these two contexts. This allowed us to compare our findings in this situation, with conditioned inhibition training in Phase 2, to prior reports of the test-context specificity of a latent inhibition treatment on subsequent excitatory conditioning that had used analogous context shifts (e.g., Bailey & Westbrook, 2008; Gray et al. 2001; Hall & Channell 1985; Kaplan & Lubow, 2001; Lovibond et al. 1984; Nakajima et al. 2006; Nelson & Sanjuan, 2006; Rosas & Bouton, 1997; Rudy, 1994; Schiller & Weiner, 2005; Zalstein-Orda & Lubow, 1995). On the basis of Bouton’s (1993) model, we expected weak conditioned inhibition to be evident in Context A relative to Context B, because both nonreinforcement (i.e., latent inhibition treatment in Context A) and a second-learned relationship concerning a cue (here, conditioned inhibition treatment in Context B) are posited to be context specific. Critically, behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition in Context B would demonstrate that the conditioned inhibition training had been effective, despite the prior latent inhibition treatment of Cue X in Context A. Given this result, we would know that any absence of evidence of conditioned inhibition in Context A was due not to a failure to acquire conditioned inhibition, but to a failure to express conditioned inhibition, at least in the present situation.

Table 2 Design summary of Experiment 2

In Phase 1, subjects received preexposure to target cue X (Groups ABB-X, ABA-X, and Sum-Con) or nontarget cue Z (Groups ABB-Con and ABA-Con) in Context A, and to nontarget cue Y in Context B (in order to equate context exposure and nonassociative experience in the two contexts). During Phase 2, all subjects received Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training with target cue X in Context B, and Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training with nontarget cue Y in Context A (in order to equate the two contexts in all manners except where the target cue training occurred during Phases 1 and 2). In Phase 3, all subjects received excitatory conditioning with cue R, the transfer CS to be used on the summation tests, in both Contexts A and B. Subjects in both the ABB and ABA conditions were tested on X for [negative] summation using transfer excitor R. The test occurred either in Context A (Condition ABA) or B (Condition ABB). The subjects in Group Summation Control (Sum-Con) were tested on N for summation using R as a transfer excitor in Context A or Context B (the two contexts were functionally equivalent for this group), in order to assess baseline behavioral control by R after controlling for any possible generalization decrement or external inhibition produced by N. Planned comparisons between Group Sum-Con and Group ABB-Con allowed us to assess the magnitude of the conditioned inhibition produced with our parameters, in the absence of associative interference or a context shift following the conditioned inhibition training. Planned comparisons between Groups ABB-X and ABB-Con and between Groups ABA-X and ABA-Con allowed us to determine the context specificity of interference with conditioned inhibition produced by the latent inhibition treatment.

Method

Subjects and apparatus

The subjects were 60 male, experimentally naive, Sprague-Dawley descended rats obtained from Harlan Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN). Their body weight range was 181–209 g at the beginning of the experiment. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of five groups (ns = 12) and were maintained and housed as in Experiment 1. The apparatuses were the same as in Experiment 1.

Procedure (see Table 2)

Acclimation

On Days 1 and 2, all subjects were given 30 min of exposure daily to Contexts A and B in a manner identical to that described in Experiment 1.

Phase 1

On Days 3–12, the five groups received exactly the same latent inhibition treatments with X or Z in Context A and Y in Context B, as in Experiment 1.

Phase 2

On Days 13–18, all groups received Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training with X in Context B and with Y in Context A. Other than this reversal of the contexts for X and Y, the conditioned inhibition training was identical to that in Experiment 1.

Phase 3

On Days 19 and 20, all subjects received R–US pairings in Contexts A and B, identical to the Phase 3 training in Experiment 1.

Reacclimation

Reacclimation to both Contexts A and B occurred on Days 21 and 22, as described in Experiment 1.

Test

On Day 23, the subjects in Conditions ABB and ABA were tested on the Compound XR in an 11-min session. The test was conducted as in Experiment 1, except that subjects in the ABB condition were tested in Context B, whereas the subjects in the ABA condition were tested in Context A. As in Experiment 1, half of the subjects in Group Sum-Con were tested in Context A and the other half in Context B, because the elements of the test compound for this group (NR) had the same history in Contexts A and B. The same elimination criterion (i.e., no more than 60 s to complete 5 s of licking prior to CS onset) that had been used in Experiment 1 was applied here.

Results and discussion

The results of Experiment 2 are depicted in Fig. 2. One subject (from Group Sum-Con) was eliminated on the basis of the elimination criterion. Two other subjects (one each in Groups ABA-Con and ABA-X) were eliminated due to experimenter errors.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Results of Experiment 2: Mean times to complete five cumulative seconds of drinking in the presence of the target test compound. Therefore, 0.7 log s is the lowest possible score. Higher scores represent stronger conditioned suppression. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. See the text and Table 2 for details

A one-way ANOVA conducted on baseline drinking (i.e., time to complete five cumulative seconds of drinking prior to CS onset) did not detect any differences among the groups, F(4, 52) = 0.76, p = .55. A similar ANOVA performed on the times to complete five cumulative seconds of drinking in the presence of the CS did reveal differences among the groups, F(4, 52) = 8.49, MSE = 0.164, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.73 (95% CI = 0.42, 1.04). Additionally, a 2 (Test Context: ABA vs. ABB) × 2 (Treatment: X vs. Con) ANOVA revealed a marginal interaction between test context and treatment, F(1, 42) = 4.06, MSE = 0.160, p = .05, Cohen’s f = 0.29 (95% CI = 0.00, 0.57). No other main effects were detected, lowest p > .09.

Planned comparisons were conducted to identify the sources of these differences and to test specific hypotheses. As expected, greater suppression was observed in Group Sum-Con than in Groups ABB-X, ABB-Con, and ABA-Con: F(1, 52) = 21.87, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.61 (95% CI = 0.33, 0.90); F(1, 52) = 19.85, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.59 (95% CI = 0.30, 0.87); and F(1, 52) = 20.58, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.58 (95% CI = 0.31, 0.88), respectively. Stronger suppression was expected in Group Sum-Con, because the transfer excitor (R) was tested in compound with N, a neutral cue that was not expected to reduce behavioral control in a summation test, other than possibly through external inhibition and/or stimulus generalization decrement. Thus, the putative conditioned inhibitor (X) attenuated suppression to R in Groups ABB-X, ABA-Con, and ABB-Con. This occurred in Group ABB-X (a) because X was tested in the context of conditioned inhibition training, with the test context presumably serving as a positive occasion setter for the expression of the second-learned conditioned inhibition training (Bouton, 1993), and/or (b) because latent inhibition failed to transfer to a context different from the one where it had been administered. Negative summation would be expected in Groups ABA-Con and ABB-Con because, for these groups, conditioned inhibition training was the first thing learned concerning X (i.e., there was no preexposure to X). Thus, there would be no ambiguity concerning what X signaled, and no basis for this learning to be context specific. Moreover, Group ABA-X exhibited marginally less suppression than Group Sum-Con, F(1, 52) = 4.00, p = .051, Cohen’s f = 0.26 (95% CI = 0.01, 0.52), suggesting that in Group ABA-X some limited amount of conditioned inhibition learning generalized from the context of conditioned inhibition training (B) to the context of the latent inhibition treatment (A). Strong suppression was observed in Group ABA-X relative to Group ABA-Con, F(1, 52) = 6.43, p = .01, Cohen’s f = 0.33 (95% CI = 0.06, 0.60), presumably because (a) in the latent inhibition context (A) the conditioned inhibition potential of X in Group ABA-X was attenuated due to Context A at test facilitating retrieval of the memory of the latent inhibition treatment, and/or (b) the conditioned inhibition acquired in Context B was context specific, due to its being learned second (Bouton, 1993). The present data do not permit us to differentiate between these two mechanisms.

More conditioned inhibition (i.e., less conditioned suppression) was observed in Group ABB-X than in Group ABA-X, F(1, 52) = 8.83, p < .01, Cohen’s f = 0.39 (95% CI = 0.12, 0.66). This is not surprising, for two reasons. First, prior research with excitation training following a latent inhibition treatment had found latent inhibition to be context specific (despite its being the first-learned information concerning X; e.g., Bailey & Westbrook, 2008; Bouton & Swartzentruber, 1989; Gray et al. 2001; Hall & Channell, 1985; Kaplan & Lubow, 2001; Lovibond et al. 1984; Maes, 2002; Nakajima et al. 2006; Nelson & Sanjuan, 2006; Rosas & Bouton, 1997; Rudy, 1994; Schiller & Weiner, 2005; Wheeler et al. 2003; Yap & Richardson, 2005; Zalstein-Orda & Lubow, 1995). Second, Group ABA-X was tested in the context of the latent inhibition treatment (A), whereas Group ABB-X was tested in the context in which conditioned inhibition training had occurred (B), which might have allowed Context B to serve as an occasion setter for X acting as a conditioned inhibitor, because in this situation conditioned inhibition was learned second (e.g., Nelson, 2002, Exp. 2; see also Sissons & Miller, 2009). Despite the prior latent inhibition treatment in Context A, the conditioned inhibition exhibited by Group ABB-X, which was tested in the context of inhibitory training (B), was sufficiently strong that suppression to XR did not significantly differ from its control ABB-Con, p = .81. This is congruent with the previously mentioned reports of latent inhibition, when followed by excitatory conditioning (as opposed to the present inhibitory conditioning), being relatively context specific.

We acknowledge that the present design does not differentiate between the strong conditioned inhibition observed in Group ABB-X, arising from the context specificity of latent inhibition, and the occasion-setting potential of the context of second learning about X (i.e., the context of conditioned inhibition training). However, unlike Experiment 1, the present data do clearly indicate that the lack of evidence of conditioned inhibition in Group ABA-X arose from the latent inhibition treatment preventing the expression of conditioned inhibition, not from preventing the acquisition of conditioned inhibition, because conditioned inhibition was observed when testing of Group ABB-X occurred in Context B following the identical treatments received by Group ABA-X. Alternatively stated, the interference with conditioned inhibition produced by the latent inhibition treatment observed in Experiment 2 was clearly a failure to express information that had been encoded in the subject. This conclusion, however, may be specific to situations like Experiment 2, in which latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training occur in different contexts.

In summary, empirically speaking, when the latent inhibition treatment was administered in one context and subsequent conditioned inhibition training occurred in a different context, the target cue (X) was a significantly more effective conditioned inhibitor on a summation test in the context of conditioned inhibition training than in the context of latent inhibition treatment. That is, the latent inhibition treatment resulted in more proactive interference with conditioned inhibition when testing occurred in the context of the latent inhibition treatment than in the context of conditioned inhibition training. Moreover, the lack of behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition observed in the context of the latent inhibition treatment was clearly not due to a failure to learn the inhibitory relationship.

General discussion

Here we conducted two experiments designed to explore the influence of the test context on interference with conditioned inhibition produced by a latent inhibition treatment, as assessed by a summation test. Both of the present experiments showed reliable evidence of conditioned inhibition in the absence of the latent inhibition treatment. In Experiment 1, administering the latent inhibition treatment prior to Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training in the same context resulted in the absence of evidence of conditioned inhibition, despite the same conditioned inhibition training being effective when there was no prior latent inhibition treatment. In Experiment 2, administering the latent inhibition treatment in one context prior to conditioned inhibition training in a different context resulted in evidence of conditioned inhibition in the context of conditioned inhibition training, but not in the context of the latent inhibition treatment. Thus, Experiment 2 demonstrated that latent inhibition treatment of a cue later used in conditioned inhibition training makes the cue’s behavioral control susceptible to contextual changes.

In the absence of the latent inhibition treatment, behavior indicative of conditioned inhibition was not context specific, in that it transferred from Context A to Context B in Experiment 1 (Group AAB-Con) and from Context B to Context A in Experiment 2 (Group ABA-Con). More interesting is what happened to the conditioned inhibition learning when it was preceded by the latent inhibition treatment, particularly as a function of the context used for testing. In Experiment 1, in which the latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training both occurred in Context A, no evidence of conditioned inhibition was observed in Context A or B, despite conditioned inhibition training without the prior latent inhibition treatment producing evidence of conditioned inhibition in both the context of the conditioned inhibition training (A) and a different context (B). However, the design of Experiment 1 did not let us determine whether the absence of evidence of conditioned inhibition in Groups AAA-X and AAB-X arose from a failure to learn during conditioned inhibition training or a failure to express conditioned inhibition learning that had occurred.

In Experiment 2, in which the latent inhibition treatment occurred in Context A and conditioned inhibition training occurred in Context B, the target cue (X) acted as a stronger conditioned inhibitor in Context B than in Context A. Thus, in Experiment 2, the test behavior was consistent with whatever had previously been learned in the test context (A or B). The results of Experiment 2 are in accord with analogous studies that have assessed the context specificity of interference by latent inhibition on subsequent excitatory conditioning (e.g., Bailey & Westbrook, 2008; Dexter & Merrill, 1969; Gray et al. 2001; Hall & Channell, 1985; Kaplan & Lubow, 2001; Lovibond et al. 1984; Nakajima et al. 2006; Nelson & Sanjuan, 2006; Rosas & Bouton, 1997; Rudy, 1994; Schiller & Weiner, 2005; Zalstein-Orda & Lubow, 1995). For example, Dexter and Merrill exposed rats to a CS in one of two Skinner boxes used for test (“modified” or “unmodified”) or in a holding cage, or gave the rats no exposure. All rats then received conditioning in a dissimilar Gerbrands rat chamber. Finally, the rats were tested in both the modified and unmodified Skinner boxes. Thus, some rats received latent inhibition treatment and testing in the same context; other rats received latent inhibition treatment and testing in two different contexts; and yet other rats received no latent inhibition treatment. Dexter and Merrill observed maximal latent inhibition effects when the CS was tested in the context of preexposure. Thus, our results are congruent with their observations, although in the present experiments the target training was inhibitory rather than excitatory, which adds generality to their findings.

One important conclusion from the present research is that at least in some circumstances (i.e., the ABA design of Exp. 2), latent inhibition of conditioned inhibition is clearly due to a retrieval failure rather than an acquisition failure, as is commonly assumed in attentional accounts of latent inhibition (e.g., Hall & Pearce, 1979). On the basis of this conclusion—and in the interests of parsimony, seeking a common mechanism for latent inhibition in all situations—one might speculate that latent inhibition generally is a form of context-specific retrieval failure (i.e., a potentially reversible deficit in behavioral control) rather than a failure to encode information concerning the Phase 2 training. This view is consistent with the findings of studies in which the Phase 2 training has consisted of excitatory conditioning rather than conditioned inhibition training (e.g., Dexter & Merrill, 1969). More broadly speaking, these observations are concordant with those found when excitatory training, rather than inhibitory training as in the present experiments, occurs during Phase 2, which lends support to the view that a common set of principles might apply across many, if not all, instances of two-phase associative interference treatment followed by a test.

On the basis of the present findings, as well as of prior data concerning the context specificity of latent inhibition followed by excitatory training (e.g., Hall & Channell, 1985) and the context specificity of conditioned inhibition when it is learned second (e.g., Nelson, 2002; Sissons & Miller, 2009), two rules are suggested concerning which of two associations will be expressed when both concern X and at least one association among the two involves the absence of an outcome (i.e., no US). First, inhibitory-like memories that are devoid of a specific US representation, such as are established by a latent inhibition treatment, are always context specific. Second, inhibitory-like associations involving the absence of an expected US, such as are established by conditioned inhibition training, appear to be context specific only when they are learned second (i.e., when conditioned inhibition training is preceded by excitatory conditioning or a latent inhibition treatment). However, the test-context specificity of conditioned inhibition trained in one context following a latent inhibition treatment in a different context has not yet been examined in a third context (i.e., an ABC paradigm). The results of such an experiment would be useful for assessing the tentative conclusions stated above.

These conclusions suggest that the memories produced by a latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training differ appreciably, with the latent inhibition memory being unique in never transferring strongly to a context other than the one in which the latent inhibition treatment was administered (weak transfer has been reported; e.g., Lovibond et al. 1984, Exp. 2). Note that this conclusion assumes that the lack of appreciable conditioned inhibition observed in Group AAB-X of the present Experiment 1, when it was tested in Context B, was due to the context specificity of second-learned conditioned inhibition rather than the transfer of latent inhibition to Context B. If it had been due to latent inhibition transferring to Context B, we might have expected latent inhibition to be evident as reduced conditioned inhibition in Group ABB-X of Experiment 2, when it was also tested in Context B, which was not observed. This distinction between the test-context specificity of latent inhibition treatment and conditioned inhibition training is not surprising, given the differences in the operations that give rise to them. Of course, this is a description of the observed behaviors, as opposed to an explanation of the underlying mechanisms that differentiate memories formed by a latent inhibition treatment from those formed by Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training.

The present observations suggest that the context of a latent inhibition treatment is a positive occasion setter for latent inhibition of the target cue, but they do not permit us to determine whether the context of conditioned inhibition training (following a latent inhibition treatment) is a positive occasion setter for conditioned inhibition of the target cue. To answer this question, one would need to use an ABC design. Both the Bouton (1993) model, in which second-learned information concerning a cue makes that specific information context specific, and the Rosas, Aguilera, Álvarez, and Abad (2006) model, in which any sort of ambiguity (which results in increased attention to the context) increases the context specificity of anything subsequently learned in the context in which the ambiguity was encountered, are in accord with the results of Experiment 2 in predicting positive occasion setting of conditioned inhibition by the context in which the conditioned inhibition training had occurred. However, in Experiment 2 we did not assess the alternative possibility of positive occasion setting of latent inhibition by the context in which the latent inhibition treatment had occurred. Moreover, assuming in Experiment 1 that conditioned inhibition was acquired but not expressed in Context A following the latent inhibition treatment, Bouton’s and Rosas et al.’s models both anticipate the present lack of evidence of conditioned inhibition in Context B, again because conditioned inhibition was the second-learned information concerning CS X. But, as we previously stated, it is unclear in this study whether conditioned inhibition was acquired after the latent inhibition treatment in the same context. To answer the question regarding whether positive occasion setting of conditioned inhibition or negative occasion setting of latent inhibition was at play in the test-context specificity observed in Experiment 2, one would need to use an ABC design. We acknowledge the absence of an ABC design as a weakness of the present report. Unfortunately, the need to equate exposure to and experience in all of the contexts in all groups made this design impractical with the present training procedures.

We emphasize that the central goal of the present report was empirical; that is, we wanted to determine the influence of the test context on interference with conditioned inhibition produced by a latent inhibition treatment. Nevertheless, we have briefly discussed several mechanisms that may have contributed the present observations. Although we are unable to fully differentiate among these mechanisms on the basis of the present data, our observations do have implications for some accounts of latent inhibition. Let us specifically consider interference models. Most interference models that account for the test-context specificity of outcome interference are based primarily on extinction data. Following excitatory conditioning in one context and extinction in a second context, what was learned during the extinction treatment appears to be relatively specific to the extinction context (e.g., Bouton & Bolles, 1979; Bouton & King, 1983). Bouton (1994) suggested that extinction memories are more context specific than are conditioning memories, either because the X–no US association is inhibitory-like or because the X–no US association presumed to result from an extinction treatment is the second information learned about X (i.e., because ambiguity about the meaning of X arises only when the extinction treatment begins, causing the extinction context to become an occasion setter for the X–no US relationship). Both of these mechanisms explain the observed test-context specificity of extinction learning. However, these two accounts diverge in their predictions for the test-context specificity of memory of a latent inhibition treatment in one context (Phase 1) followed by excitatory conditioning in a second context (Phase 2). Many researchers have reported that after a latent inhibition treatment in one context and conditioned excitation in a second context, the excitatory memory generalizes to other test contexts relative to testing back in the latent inhibition context (e.g., Lovibond et al. 1984; Westbrook et al. 2000). Thus, in the case of a latent inhibition treatment, the CS–no US memory is context specific, rather than the CS–US memory. In contrast, on the basis of the existing evidence, it appears that the second-learned association is context specific when Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training either precedes or follows conditioned excitation training (Nelson, 2002; Sissons & Miller, 2009). Nelson, as well as Sissons and Miller, observed that Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training in Phase 2 was specific to the context of conditioned inhibition training when Phase 1 had consisted of excitatory conditioning.

Comparing the present observations to the aforementioned outcome interference situations, at least one generality emerges. In Experiment 2, in which latent inhibition and conditioned inhibition treatments were administered in different contexts, the interference effect of latent inhibition on conditioned inhibition was stronger when the test context matched the one in which the latent inhibition treatment had occurred. This indicates that similarity between the test context and the training context influences the expression of the association trained in that context, a result that is identical to the context specificity of the outcome interference effect observed in ABA renewal following both extinction (e.g., Bouton & King, 1983) and counterconditioning (Peck & Bouton, 1990).

We close by noting that most models of associative interference have been devised to explain only a limited number of specific instances of associative interference. For example, Bouton’s (1993) account does well with outcome interference, but it fails to speak to cue interference (e.g., X–Outcome followed Y–Outcome; Escobar, Matute, & Miller, 2001; Matute & Pineño, 1998). In contrast, Miller and Escobar (2002) proposed an account of associative interference that addresses both outcome interference and cue interference. Of course, outcome interference and cue interference may possibly arise from different underlying mechanisms. Notably, Miller and Escobar’s account of interference has much in common with the various accounts of so-called retrieval-induced forgetting in humans (e.g., Anderson & Spellman, 1995; Jonker, Seli, & MacLeod, 2013), which is clearly a variant of retroactive outcome interference (in the sense that the term has been used in this article; i.e., X–C training, then X–D training, followed by a test of X–C; see especially the use of the “extralist paradigm” in retrieval-induced forgetting; e.g., Bäuml, 2002; Storm, Bjork, Bjork, & Nestojko, 2006). However, retrieval-induced forgetting is seldom related to more traditional associative interference situations (but see Miguez, Mash, Polack, & Miller, 2014; Vadillo et al. 2013). Thus, there appears to be a need for researchers to seek commonalities (and differences) across different instances of associative interference, rather than simply staying focused on a single instance.