Introduction

Affective and associative processes are both crucial for an individual’s ability to understand and act in the world. To best anticipate how to respond to a newly presented object, the brain immediately begins the process of object recognition, while also extracting the motivational relevance (or affective value) of that object. In so doing, the brain quickly and efficiently activates relevant associations that give rise to focused predictions (i.e., “what other objects or contexts might go with this object?”; Bar, 2004, 2009; Chun & Jiang, 2003; Oliva & Torralba, 2007). The ease with which a stimulus brings to mind other related stimuli and contexts (what we will refer to as its associativity)Footnote 1 is central to research into memory, prospection, imagination, and scene construction (Bar, 2004; Bar, Aminoff, Mason, & Fenske, 2007; Barsalou, 2009; Bartlett, 1932; Bower, 2008; Eichenbaum & Fortin, 2009; James, 1890). Similarly, the brain quickly predicts an object’s affective value—in particular, its valence (i.e., “is this something pleasant/approachable or unpleasant/to-be-avoided?”; Barrett & Bliss-Moreau, 2009; Cabanac, 2002; Damasio, 1994; Rolls, 1986; Russell, 2003). These two domains of prediction are supported by vast yet largely nonoverlapping psychological and neuroscientific literatures, despite indications that they might share some cognitive and neural mechanisms (Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, 2010; Barrett & Bar, 2009; D'Argembeau et al., 2009). One region in particular that is consistently implicated in both fields of research is the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC; meta-analytic summary in Roy, Shohamy, & Wager, 2012).

Cognitive neuroscientific research has shown that regions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC; including the mOFC) are involved in tasks that engage associative processing, including those that do so through recall of past autobiographical experiences (e.g., Burianova & Grady, 2007; Denkova, Botzung, Scheiber, & Manning, 2006), imagination of possible future events (e.g., Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007; Peters & Buchel, 2010; Szpunar, Watson, & McDermott, 2007), mind-wandering (e.g., Christoff, Gordon, Smallwood, Smith, & Schooler, 2009; Mason et al., 2007), and perception of contextually associative stimuli (objects/scenes; e.g., Bar, 2004; Bar & Aminoff, 2003). These research areas have shown that the mOFC is more active as tasks elicit greater associative processing (reviewed in Bar et al., 2007; Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter, 2008; for a related meta-analytic summary, see Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2009).

Other areas of human neuroscience have extensively explored the mOFC’s role in affective processing. This has been studied in the context of perception for both basic stimuli (e.g., tastes, smells; Grabenhorst & Rolls, 2008; Rolls, Kringelbach, & de Araujo, 2003) and complex stimuli (e.g., emotional faces, affect-laden scenes; Nielen et al., 2009; O'Doherty, Winston, et al., 2003), extinction of conditioned emotional responses (e.g., Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007), choices between different magnitudes and types of rewarding and/or aversive stimuli (e.g., Kim, Shimojo, & O'Doherty, 2006; O'Doherty, Kringelbach, Rolls, Hornak, & Andrews, 2001), and updating of associated outcome expectations over time (e.g., Daw, O'Doherty, Dayan, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006; Hampton, Bossaerts, & O'Doherty, 2006; Seymour et al., 2005). Across these domains, mOFC activity typically correlates with whether and/or to what degree a stimulus is more pleasant or preferred (i.e., more positively valenced; meta-analytic summaries in Brown, Gao, Tisdelle, Eickhoff, & Liotti, 2011; Grabenhorst & Rolls, 2011; Kringelbach & Berridge, 2009; Liu, Hairston, Schrier, & Fan, 2011).

Because these areas of research traditionally take place relatively independently of one another and with different types of stimuli/paradigms, the intersection between affective and associative processes and the shared neural architecture that underlies them remain poorly understood. This is important not only because it represents a crucial gap in our understanding of how these systems and processes interact, but also because it introduces some degree of ambiguity when interpreting the results of a given study. Specifically, the centrality of the mOFC to a large body of research in these two separate fields, combined with the fact that both contextual associations and affective value appear to be processed automatically in perception and cognition (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999; Zajonc, 1980), makes it possible that studies intending only to manipulate affect observed activation in the mOFC related to the associative nature of their stimuli but attributed it to affect and vice versa (cf. Peters, 2011). We were therefore particularly interested in elucidating whether the mOFC’s presumed stimulus processing role in one of these dimensions is accounted for by the other.

To address this key issue, in the present study, we examined both affective and associative processing in the mOFC within a single experiment, using stimuli that varied parametrically along each of these dimensions. Specifically, using a single paradigm, we tested whether the same region of the mOFC would show increased activity in response to objects with greater positive affective value (higher valence), as well as to objects that are better able to elicit associations (higher associativity). One possibility is that the mOFC performs a processing role corresponding exclusively to either affective or associative processing. If this is the case, our experiment should confirm that mOFC activity is entirely accounted for by either valence or associativity, suggesting that it is possible to reduce one psychological domain to the other. A second possibility is that the mOFC is a hetero-functional region that separately processes information along these different dimensions or performs a more general function to which both affective and associative information contribute. In this case, the mOFC might track each object dimension independently, resulting in a purely additive influence of the two in BOLD signal measured within the mOFC. A third possibility is that mOFC responsiveness to valence and associativity might be dominated by an interaction between these two dimensions.

We first replicate previously separate sets of results showing BOLD activity in the mOFC tracking positive value and increasing associativity simultaneously during object perception. Importantly, we extend these findings to show that, from the perspective of mOFC activity, neither of these dimensions is reducible to the other; valence and associativity are independently and additively related to activity in the mOFC. Finally, we provide preliminary evidence suggesting that associativity-selectivity and valence-selectivity, at the voxel level, may not simply arise from separate regions within the mOFC.

Method

Subjects

Twenty-three healthy right-handed subjects with no reported history of neurological or affective disorders and normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and color discrimination abilities were recruited for the fMRI experiment. Three subjects were excluded for excessive missed trials (>30 % of total trials), and 1 for an incomplete session. Thus, these results will reflect analysis of 19 total subjects (12 female; age, 19–36 years; mean age, 23.8). Written informed consent was obtained prior to the start of the scanning session, in accordance with a Human Studies Protocol (#2001P-001754) approved by Massachusetts General Hospital.

Image acquisition

Images were acquired using a Siemens 3 T Trio Tim MR magnet and a 32-channel RF head coil. We acquired functional image volumes as T2*-weighted echo-planar images (EPIs) with the following parameters: 36 interleaved slices, 2,200-ms TR, 28-ms TE, 2.5-mm thickness, 0.75-mm gap, 64 × 64 matrix, 200-mm FOV (resulting in an inplane voxel size of 3.125 × 3.125 × 2.5 mm). Our fMRI sequence and slice prescription was optimized for reducing signal loss and distortion in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), including the use of a modified z-shim prepulse moment and 30° tilt of our slice prescription counterclockwise of the AC/PC line along the sagittal plane (based on recommendations of Deichmann, Gottfried, Hutton, & Turner, 2003). As a consequence of the limited slice prescription used in order to achieve optimal OFC signal, the most dorsal portions of the posterior parietal cortex were not captured in the scan volume for a majority of subjects (13 out of 19). Each subject performed three functional runs, each consisting of 149 TRs. Each run included 11 s of fixation at the beginning (to allow for the fMRI signal to reach steady-state), and the corresponding five EPI volumes were discarded from further analysis. Each session included the acquisition of two high-resolution T1-weighted Multi-Echo MPRAGE anatomical images (1-mm isotropic voxels), which were later averaged together.

Stimulus norming

Our analyses explored three categories of valence (negative, neutral, positive) and two categories of associativity (weak and strong). Our first stage of analysis focused on replicating previously separate findings in as broad a stimulus set as possible. For this stage, associativity analyses were therefore limited to neutrally valenced objects previously normed by Bar and Aminoff (2003). Our second stage of analysis was finer-grained and used object categories that fully crossed levels of valence and associativity, based on stimulus renorming with a large group of raters. (See also the Supplementary Discussion regarding motivation for and potential limitations of using independent ratings.)

Valence

Images of strongly and weakly associative objects were initially compiled from a set used in Bar and Aminoff (2003; available at http://barlab.mgh.harvard.edu/ContextLocalizer.htm), as well as additional stimuli previously compiled using similar norming procedures. These neutrally valenced objects were supplemented with images of positive and negative affect-inducing objects compiled and modified primarily from the International Affective Picture System (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008), as well as from online image searches. Each of the resulting 462 objects was viewed by 7 independent raters (14 raters total, each viewing half of the set), who provided valence ratings on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from very unpleasant to very pleasant (centered on neutral). The averaged ratings and their variances were used to limit this set to objects consistently rated as positive (pleasant) and negative (unpleasant). These ratings were also used to limit objects previously categorized as strongly/weakly associative to those consistently rated as neutral. After further limiting the set to semantically nonoverlapping stimuli, the resulting set of stimuli included 276 objects—69 each of positive (valence M ± SD: 5.71 ± 0.43), negative (2.18 ± 0.39), weakly associative neutral (4.14 ± 0.45), and strongly associative neutral (4.18 ± 0.45). This set of 276 objects was used in the first stage of analysis. To allow for the second stage of analysis, each of these objects was renormed for valence by an average of 39 new raters (range: 34–43), using the same procedure as above, and the stimulus set was further limited to orthogonalize these ratings against ratings of associativity collected for the same objects (as described in the following section; see also Table 1).

Table 1 Means (standard deviations) for valence ratings (top) and associativity index (middle) after controlling for associativity (across valence categories) and valence (across associativity categories)

Associativity

The set of 276 objects described above was presented to an independent set of raters to determine the ease with which each image elicited associations with other objects. Each image was presented to an average of 39 raters (range: 35–47; 116 individuals each viewed approximately one third of the total image set). Each image was presented separately, and while it remained on the screen, raters were asked to try their best to type the names of three separate objects that they associated with it. For instance, an image of a washing machine might elicit “detergent,” “clothes,” and “dryer” as associates. Raters’ response time on this task was not limited, and they were required to produce at least one associate per image. This procedure allowed us to derive our key measures of associativity for a given image, including the number of times that an object failed to produce all three associates (i.e., the proportion of associate slots left unfilled for a given object across subjects; ease/difficulty of association) and the response time (RT) to associate production (normalized for both word length and each rater’s mean typing rate; speed of association). These two measures—ease and speed of association—were significantly correlated across objects, r(276) = .53, p < .001, and so were normalized and averaged together to form a single associativity index (with higher values representing objects that produced associations more easily and faster—thus, strongly associative). If, in the example above, the washing machine consistently produced only one or two associates and/or those associates took a long time to be generated, its associativity index would be low, and we would describe it as weakly associative. If the reverse were true (i.e., all three associates were consistently given and relatively quickly), it would be considered strongly associative. While some raters participated in both valence and associativity norming, a given rater viewed each object only once, providing either a valence or an associativity rating for that object.

For both analysis stages, these variables are discretized into categorical rather than continuous variables. The motivation for this was twofold. First, this is the approach used by a number of studies whose findings we were most closely attempting to replicate, for both associativity (e.g., Bar & Aminoff, 2003) and valence (e.g., Nielen et al., 2009; Ritchey et al., 2011; Sass et al., 2011). Second, because valence and associativity turned out to be intercorrelated (see the Results section), the second set of analyses tested for fully independent effects of the two object dimension on mOFC activity by forming stimulus categories matched along the potentially confounding dimension. This approach to orthogonalizing our variables of interest, which we determined to have less potential of being overly conservative than alternatives, partially guided the decision to use categorical variables for all of our main analyses (see also the Supplementary Analysis).

Task design

The paradigm employed a rapid event-related design. Target images were color photographs (256 × 256 pixels) of objects in isolation presented on a white background. Each target image was presented briefly (150 ms) and was immediately followed by a colorful mask presented for 100 ms (see Fig. 1). This was done in order to encourage the subject to bring to mind the object and its internal associations (affective and contextual) and minimize the degree to which the subject was modulating attention toward the image of the object in front of them and the individual features thereof, as well as to encourage the subject to answer relatively automatically, rather than to consciously deliberate over potential responses. A red fixation cross then appeared signaling the start of the response period and turned black after 1,500 ms, signaling the end of the response period. The black fixation cross remained on the screen for the duration of the intertrial interval (ITI), which ranged from 200 to 9,250 ms (to allow a jittered ITI in multiples of TR length and jittered stimulus presentation from the start of each TR). The fMRI session consisted of 276 unique trials, pseudorandomly ordered across three functional runs, in addition to 28 practice trials (practice trials presented objects not included in the main task). Each target image was presented only once.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Task timeline. Subjects viewed objects presented briefly and in isolation (150 ms), followed by a colorful backward mask (100 ms), and then rated how common the object was (1.5-s response period). In between trials, subjects viewed a black fixation cross for variable durations

Subjects were instructed to rate how common each object was on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all common to extremely common. This task was chosen in order to require some high level of object recognition without focusing attention explicitly on either affective or associative qualities of the object, an assumption we later verify by comparing ratings of commonality across image categories. Responses were provided on a five-button MR-compatible response box. The order of the 5-point scale was counterbalanced across subjects to prevent confounds between rating and motor mapping, and subjects practiced the appropriate mapping to proficiency before the practice trials began. Stimulus presentation and response collection was performed using Psychtoolbox (http://www.psychtoolbox.org; Brainard, 1997) running on MATLAB (http://www.mathworks.com), controlled by a MacBook Pro laptop with a monitor resolution of 1,024 × 768 and a refresh rate of 60 Hz.

fMRI analysis

Structural and functional imaging analyses were performed using the Freesurfer and FS-FAST analysis tools and processing stream developed at the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). Data from individual fMRI runs were first motion corrected using the AFNI motion correction algorithm (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni/), in which all images were aligned to the first image of the first functional run. The data were then spatially smoothed using a Gaussian full-width at half-max (FWHM) of 6 mm. The first five volumes were removed from each fMRI run to allow for signal stabilization. The intensities for all runs were globally rescaled such that the in-brain mean intensity was 1,000. Signal intensity for each condition was then computed and averaged throughout all the runs. Each subject’s fMRI volumes were also co-registered to their own high-resolution structural volume, and a semi-automated procedure was used to segment this structural data into gray/white matter components and to extract the outer cortical surface for each hemisphere as a topologically preserved spherical representation (software and documentation is available at http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/freesurfer).

For the first analysis stage, all events were modeled according to the four object categories, and for the second analysis stage, an additional condition modeled any stimuli excluded from the six more conservatively defined categories. Analyses reported include any trials where subjects failed to respond in time (which were infrequent and not influenced by object category; see the Behavioral Results section), but all results remain unchanged if these are modeled as a separate condition (see also the Supplementary Analysis). The estimated hemodynamic response was defined by a gamma function of 2.25-s hemodynamic delay and 1.25-s dispersion. Data were then tested for statistical significance for each individual (first level), and contrast maps were constructed comparing the BOLD estimates for each condition. While the a priori focus of our analyses was on a specific set of regions of interest (ROIs; described below), to allow comparison with previous findings and test for overlap between key contrasts, whole-brain random effects analyses were performed at the group (second) level with an omnibus F-test and individual t-tests over contrast maps generated at the first level. In order to reduce the influence of noise at the first level, these random effects analyses weighted first-level contrast maps by the inverse of their variance (Thirion et al., 2007). Whole-brain statistical maps were corrected for multiple comparisons using a cluster significance threshold of p < .05 (corrected), with a cluster-defining (voxelwise) threshold of p < .01. Since primary analyses are restricted to a priori ROIs, we chose a standard but relatively liberal cluster-defining threshold in order to describe the full extent of our network of activations. However, because cluster correction was performed in volumetric space, activations shown on the cortical surface (Fig. 3, left) are not cluster corrected but, instead, set at a more conservative voxelwise threshold of p < .001. All whole-brain analyses were performed for confirmatory rather than exploratory reasons (i.e., ROI selection was based strictly on anatomy, rather than functional activation). In order to visualize overlap between individual contrasts, a conjunction was performed over these corrected maps using the minimum statistic (Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager, & Poline, 2005). While ROI analyses were performed on cortical surfaces in each subject’s native space, group-level analyses were performed after transforming each subject’s data into normalized (Talairach) space.

ROI analysis

Given a priori predictions about the involvement of the two regions in our task, we used Freesurfer’s automated segmentation and parcellation algorithms (Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004) to define anatomical ROIs within the left and right mOFC on the basis of each subject’s individual anatomy (see Fig. 3b for an example). The parcellation algorithms used gyral and sulcal landmarks from each individual’s surface anatomy to define an ROI on each hemisphere that extended rostrally/caudally according to the boundaries of the medial orbital gyrus, was bounded on the orbital surface by the midpoint of the olfactory sulcus, and was bounded on the medial surface by the inferior boundaries of cingulate and superior frontal gyri (Desikan et al., 2006). Averaged beta weights were extracted from this ROI for each of our conditions and then converted to percent signal change values. In order to determine whether BOLD activity in this region tracked one or both of our parameters of interest (or their interaction), these values were then entered into a mixed-effects valence × associativity ANOVA model, with subject as a random effect. We used a two-tailed p-value less than .025 (p < .05, corrected for the left- and right-hemisphere a priori ROIs) as our significance threshold in these ROI analyses, and we note the cases in which this correction was applied when reporting the results of the ROI analyses.

Voxel overlap analysis

In order to perform a more conservative test against the possibility that valence and associativity sensitivity revealed in mOFC voxels was actually occurring in segregated sets of voxels within our anatomically defined ROI, we extracted averaged percent BOLD signal change estimates for each valence condition from the 20 most associativity-selective voxels in the left mOFC. Selectivity was assessed on a within-subjects basis by rank-ordering voxels (without consideration for spatial contiguity) on the basis of p-values from the omnibus F-test for a voxel-wise general linear model (GLM) that included only category labels for strong and weak associativity. We then tested for a significant effect of valence category in these associativity-selective voxels, using a mixed-effects ANOVA. We also performed the reverse analysis, testing for associativity selectivity in the most valence-selective voxels. To test for robustness, this analysis was repeated for samples of between 1 and 100 peak voxels.

Results

Subjects performed a simple task requiring the recognition of visually presented objects while undergoing fMRI scans (Fig. 1). Images of isolated objects were presented briefly (150 ms), and subjects were asked to judge how common the object was. All objects were independently rated for hedonic valence and associativity, and planned ROI analyses tested for changes in mOFC BOLD signal across levels of valence and associativity. Our first stage of analysis focuses on four object categories (negative, neutral–weak, neutral–strong, positive; for examples, see Fig. 2a), defined across the full set of stimuli viewed in the scanner. Our second stage narrows this stimulus set to allow for fully crossed and orthogonalized analyses of all six possible categories (negative–weak, negative–strong, neutral–weak, neutral–strong, positive–weak, positive–strong; Fig. 4a).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Analysis of mOFC reactivity to affectively valenced categories, relative to neutral objects of varying associativity. a Examples of stimuli from each of the four object categories. b Individually defined left mOFC anatomical ROI shown on a single subject’s inflated surface. c Group ROI analyses show average BOLD response to each of these four categories in the left mOFC. Error bars reflect between-subjects standard errors of the means

Behavioral results

Subject ratings and RTs were entered into separate mixed-effect ANOVAs, with subject as a random effect. On the basis of the four initial categories, we found a significant overall main effect of condition on ratings of commonality, F(3, 54) = 101.2, p < .001, such that negative objects were consistently rated less common, on average, than objects in the other three conditions (see the Supplementary Materials for secondary analyses controlling for this). There was no significant difference between commonality ratings for the remaining three conditions on the basis of post hoc contrasts, F(3, 100) = 0.92, p > .40. There was no significant main effect of condition on RT, F(3, 54) = 2.0, p > .10, suggesting no clear difference in task difficulty across the four conditions. The lack of commonality differences (with the exception of negative objects) and lack of RT differences remained true after trials were later resorted for the second analysis stage (see Table 1 legend). Subjects missed relatively few trials (median = 2.2 %, mean ± SD = 5.7 % ± 6.9 %), with no effect of condition, F(3, 54) < 0.10.

An additional finding emerged related to the independent ratings of our two object dimensions of interest. We found that an object’s valence and degree of associativity were moderately correlated, r(276) = .31, p < .0001 (see Supplementary Fig. 1), such that objects that were rated as more positive tended to be more strongly associative. While this relationship is interesting in its own right and further motivates the need for disambiguation of previous mOFC findings, we also control for this as a potential confound in our second set of analyses.

Imaging results: replication of previously separate affective and associative findings

Our analyses focused on left and right mOFC a priori ROIs using surface anatomic landmarks in each subject’s native space (see the Method section and the example in Fig. 2b). Since we had no clear a priori hypothesis about one hemisphere or another, our ROI analyses were Bonferroni-corrected for the dual a priori hypotheses. We extracted estimates of BOLD signal from these mOFC ROIs for each of the four broader stimulus categories and found a significant main effect of condition [Fig. 2c; left, F(3, 54) = 19.3; right, F(3, 54) = 8.52; ps < .001, corrected]. A series of planned post hoc contrasts confirmed that both associativity and affective value engaged the mOFC. Directly replicating previous studies of associativity (Aminoff, Schacter, & Bar, 2008; Bar & Aminoff, 2003), we found that neutral objects with strong associations significantly increased BOLD activity in the mOFC, when compared with weakly associative objects [left, F(1, 54) = 33.3; right, F(1, 54) = 15.4; ps < .001, corrected]. Mirroring findings for a number of different kinds of affectively valenced stimuli (Brown et al., 2011; Chib, Rangel, Shimojo, & O'Doherty, 2009; Lebreton, Jorge, Michel, Thirion, & Pessiglione, 2009; Nielen et al., 2009; O'Doherty, Critchley, Deichmann, & Dolan, 2003; Sass et al., 2011), BOLD activity in the mOFC also increased significantly for positive objects, as compared with the neutrally valenced objects (i.e., neutral–strong and neutral–weak combined) and with the negative objects [left, F(3, 72) = 8.2, p < .001, corrected; right, F(3, 72) = 3.4, p < .05, corrected; Fig. 2c].

While our analyses focus explicitly on individually defined ROIs, results of a confirmatory group whole-brain analysis (Fig. 3, Supplementary Table 1) show that activations in the mOFC appear prominently in each of the three whole-brain corrected contrasts of interest. These analyses also reveal expected patterns of coactivation for associativity in retrosplenial and parahippocampal cortices, which have been observed across all previous studies of associativity (Bar et al., 2007) and many studies of episodic and autobiographical memory more generally (Buckner et al., 2008; Roy et al., 2012). And regions coactivated with the mOFC in valence contrasts, including the dorsal mPFC and amygdala, are also commonly coactivated with this region across studies of affective processing (Kober et al., 2008; Roy et al., 2012). A conjunction of these contrasts confirmed that the greatest degree of spatial overlap fell within the mOFC and, in particular, the left mOFC (Fig. 3e). One immediate implication of these results is that the differential activation in the mOFC to strongly versus weakly associative objects reported in previous research is robust to more careful control for valence. However, this still leaves unanswered the question of whether the effect of valence on the mOFC is mediated by associativity. We turn to this question next in our second analysis stage. Because our investigation was primarily concerned not with the function of all of the mOFC but, specifically, with those regions related to both affect and associations, the remaining analyses focus on the left mOFC, where selectivity was substantially stronger for both dimensions [as discussed above and further confirmed by a significant ROI × condition interaction: F(3, 54) = 10.13, p < .0001]. Bonferroni correction continued to be applied to adjust for our two a priori hypotheses.

Fig. 3
figure 3

a Whole-brain analyses projected onto the inflated cortical surface display the main effect of condition (left; significance values based on F-test for group repeated measures ANOVA) and the simple effect contrast (t-test) for the average of all conditions, relative to baseline (right). bd Whole-brain contrasts (t-tests) display relative BOLD activation when viewing strongly versus weakly associative objects (neutrally valenced) and objects with positive affective value (relative to neutral and negative) (left: surface-based maps shown at uncorrected voxelwise threshold of p < .001; right: same activations shown on the brain volume in Talairach space at voxelwise p < .01, extent-thresholded to achieve a whole-brain cluster-corrected threshold of p < .05). e A conjunction of these three (corrected) contrasts reveals a role for the left mOFC at the intersection of affective and associative processing. Whole-brain maps provided for completeness, but analyses of interest were performed in a priori ROIs

Imaging results: relationship between affective and associative processing in the mOFC

Subsequent analyses revealed that the effects of valence and associativity on mOFC activity are independent of and additive with one another, indicating that the mOFC’s role in affective processing is not attributable to associativity or vice versa. To confirm that mOFC activity was independently influenced by both object properties, this more conservative second stage of analysis focused on a subset of objects that were classified into one of six possible combinations of valence and associativity level (negative–weak, etc.). These objects were also selected so that categories at the same level of associativity were matched for valence and vice versa, to address any potential confounds between the two (see Table 1 and examples in Fig. 4a). We were then able to run a 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (associativity: strong vs. weak) ANOVA for BOLD signal in the left mOFC. We found that left mOFC BOLD activity revealed main effects of valence [F(2, 36) = 4.88, p < .05, corrected] and associativity [F(1, 18) = 25.7, p < .0005, corrected; valence × associativity: F(2, 36) = 2.70, p > .15, corrected], indicating that both properties contributed to independent increases in mOFC activity (Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Analysis of mOFC reactivity to fully crossed set of objects of increasing valence (negative, neutral, positive) and different associativity levels (weak vs. strong). a Examples of stimuli from each of six object categories. b Group ROI analyses in the left mOFC reveal main effects for both valence and associativity. Error bars reflect between-subjects standard errors of the means

The results thus far suggest that activity in the mOFC independently increases with positive affective value and increased associativity. One possibility that remains is that these results arise from two segregated but partially overlapping regions within the left mOFC, one selective only for valence and the other selective only for associativity. This hypothesis cannot be disconfirmed by comparing peak coordinates or exploring activation overlap at differing thresholds, because it is always possible that voxels most selective for one parameter are still significantly selective for the other parameter but that their significance fails to meet the given threshold. In order to provide evidence to militate against the possibility that our independent effects of affect and associativity originate from segregated regions within the mOFC, we instead identified the 20 voxels within the mOFC that were most selective for associativity (on a within-subjects basis) and extracted from each subject the average percent BOLD signal change for each of the valence conditions. A mixed-effects ANOVA (treating subject as a random effect) revealed a significant effect of valence within these voxels, F(2, 36) = 4.98, p < .02. We then did the same for BOLD estimates of strongly versus weakly associative objects within voxels most selective for valence and again found a marginally significant effect for associativity, F(1, 18) = 4.16, p < .06 (Supplementary Fig. 2a).Footnote 2 To ensure that neither set of estimates benefited from error variance accounted for by the other parameter, voxel selection and beta estimate extraction were derived from statistically independent GLMs over the same data set (one GLM accounting only for levels of associativity and the other only levels of valence). These findings provide preliminary support for the hypothesis that overlapping populations of voxels within the mOFC were independently responsive to both an object’s affective value and the strength of its associativity with other objects/contexts.

Discussion

The mOFC has been implicated in research on both affective and associative processes, raising the important question of whether one set of proposed functions better accounts for the other. In the present study, we measured mOFC BOLD activity while subjects viewed objects that were varied orthogonally in affective valence and associativity. Replicating findings from separate literatures, we demonstrated that activity in the same region of the left mOFC was proportional to an object’s valence (specifically, whether it was positively valenced or not; Brown et al., 2011; Grabenhorst & Rolls, 2011; Kringelbach & Berridge, 2009) and to its associative strength (Aminoff et al., 2008; Bar & Aminoff, 2003). Crucially, we found that activation in this mOFC region was not reducible to either valence or associativity, even after controlling for interdependencies between the two dimensions. Both properties independently and additively modulated left mOFC activity, despite the fact that neither was integral to the task at hand. Moreover, sensitivity to an object’s affective value and its associative strength seemed to arise from overlapping rather than separate populations of voxels within the anatomically defined mOFC.Footnote 3

In terms of both structure and connectivity, it is not surprising that the mOFC seems important for both contextually associative and affective predictions. The mOFC’s proposed role in object recognition is supported by its strong connectivity with magnocellular visual processing regions along the dorsal stream (Barbas, 2007a, 2007b; Carmichael & Price, 1995b); the mOFC also sends projections back to the ventral visual stream, potentially influencing early stages of object processing in the inferotemporal cortex (Kveraga, Ghuman, & Bar, 2007; Kveraga et al., 2011; Summerfield et al., 2006). The mOFC has also been described as a center of integration for information regarding affective significance or value, and it sends and receives projections from regions involved in processing and regulating the autonomic physiology associated with motivated states, including the amygdala, hypothalamus, cingulate cortex, and brainstem, as well as indirect projections from the ventral striatum (Carmichael & Price, 1995a; Öngür & Price, 2000). Furthermore, because the mOFC receives afferent projections from all sensory modalities (largely via its dense connections with lateral aspects of the OFC), it is optimally situated for linking representations to inform judgments (Grabenhorst & Rolls, 2011; Murray & Wise, 2010; Wallis, 2007).

Taken together, previous research suggests that the mOFC engages in predictive processing across both cognitive and affective domains by allowing for associations between a given stimulus and both its higher-order sensory relations (i.e., other stimuli/contexts) and affective outcomes (i.e., valence) to be rapidly computed and used for the proactive generation of the corresponding predictions. One possible interpretation of the present results is, therefore, that the mOFC plays different roles for a number of independent psychological functions, or that its role is fundamentally associative but that these associations take on a number of different forms. This model gains support from the fact that the diverse afferents to the mOFC described above make it an ideal information-processing hub for independently collecting and processing information related to stimulus valence (e.g., from the lateral OFC and the amygdala) and to associated stimuli and contexts in memory (e.g., from the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex). Under this interpretation, our results would be a natural consequence of this region’s putative role in separately maintaining and/or transferring information related to stimulus associations, defined broadly to include associations between stimuli and other stimuli/contexts and associations between stimuli and outcome values.

In contrast, another equally plausible interpretation is that the convergence of pleasantness and associativity within the mOFC suggests that this brain area subserves a more unified purpose to which both of those psychological properties relate. The affective and reinforcement literature has suggested that such a unitary function is generating positive (and/or negative; Damasio, 1994, 1996) affective states and/or encoding positive affective value in a stimulus (Kringelbach & Berridge, 2009; Lebreton et al., 2009; Rolls, 1986). Building in part on psychological mechanisms previously theorized to underlie emotion (Barrett, 2006; Cabanac, 2002; Damasio, 1994; Rolls, 1986; Russell, 2003; Weiskrantz, 1968), recent research into decision making and reinforcement learning has suggested that the kind of affective value encoded by the mOFC represents the expected value of options under consideration and can be more appropriately considered as an abstract “currency” for guiding decisions (Haber & Knutson, 2010; Kable & Glimcher, 2009; Rangel & Hare, 2010; Wallis, 2007). From this perspective, our findings would raise the important question of whether determining an object’s overall expected value may include/require consideration of both its affective value and its “associative value” (i.e., the ease or multiplicity of association between it and other objects/contexts in memory). This estimate of associativity can aid the expected value computation in a number of possible ways, including as a proxy measure of possible states and physical actions that must be evaluated in relation to this stimulus (Rangel, Camerer, & Montague, 2008). From the perspective of the processes of recall and prospection, a stimulus may also gain some currency for downstream information processing if it is better able to link up stimuli in memory or allow fluid production of future plans through its promiscuous binding to other stimuli (i.e., a type of informational processing fluency; cf. Kurth-Nelson, Bickel, & Redish, 2012; Murray & Wise, 2010). In other words, a stimulus might be encoded as more valuable merely because it elicits a large number of associations (Bar, Shenhav, & Devaney, 2012). For the same reason, mOFC activity may increase with the meaningfulness (and identifiability) of an object (Chaumon, Kveraga, Barrett, & Bar, 2012) because of the associative activation that a meaningful object elicits. While the exact kind of value evinced by associativity is still a matter of speculation, if it turns out to be the case that object associativity is another kind of value being encoded in the mOFC, one would indeed expect to see the independent encoding for valence and associativity found in our data.

Our data and the interpretations offered above are also broadly consistent with a recent review by Roy et al. (2012). In integrating across research on memory and affect, the authors proposed that the overarching function of this region (specifically, the ventromedial PFC) may be described as determining a stimulus’ “affective meaning,” combining both conceptual and affective inputs toward this goal (e.g., Kumaran, Summerfield, Hassabis, & Maguire, 2009). Under this view, the mOFC may integrate value representations with the actual stimuli/contexts associated with an object, consistent with the mOFC’s proposed role in representing specific states and stimulus contingencies relevant to learned reward values (i.e., "model-based" learning; Bornstein, Nylen, & Steele, 2011; Schoenbaum, Takahashi, Liu, & Mcdannald, 2011). The result would be increased mOFC activity for increasing numbers of readily available stimuli/contexts (i.e., stronger associativity). In this respect, future studies should consider whether functional connectivity between mOFC and regions coactivated by increasing associativity (e.g., parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices) predicts the degree to which choices made in the scanner reflect model-based versus model-free learning and inference (Daw et al., 2011). It would also be interesting to see whether the reverse is true for mOFC connectivity with regions like the amygdala.

It further remains to be determined how value-related encoding in the mOFC influences cognitive processing in general and the process of object association in particular. Our finding that stimulus pleasantness and associativity are correlated suggests that one possible outcome of mOFC valuation might be to modulate the extent of associative processing for a given stimulus (Bar, 2009). In this view, positive affective states are linked with disinhibited association (i.e., a greater readiness to form or activate associations between stimuli), and negative affective states are directly related to the inhibition of stimulus–stimulus associations. While the associations attached to a stimulus are more likely to be stored in the medial temporal cortex and/or sensory cortices, according to this model mOFC could exert inhibitory control over how much associative activation is afforded downstream (Bar, 2009). In this kind of model, the mOFC would be involved in overseeing the scope of associative processing such that positively valenced signals would trigger a broadening of this scope and signals indicating negative valence in an object would result in narrowing this scope through inhibition of associative processing by the mOFC (cf. Sass et al., 2011), similar to what is seen in the narrowing of attentional focus for negatively valenced stimuli (Baddeley, 1972; Gasper & Clore, 2002). While still largely hypothetical, this model gains support from recent findings in rhesus macaques showing that the nearby perigenual anterior cingulate cortex (BA 32) projects to regions of the parahippocampal cortex in ways that would allow it to exert excitatory and inhibitory control over the local information-processing circuitry (Bunce & Barbas, 2011). Moreover, previous behavioral studies have identified a link between positive mood states and the generation of broader associations (Brunye et al., 2012; Clore & Huntsinger, 2007; Fredrickson, 2004; Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985; Mason & Bar, 2011).

Our study carries broad implications for research into affective as well as associative processing. In particular, future studies focused on mOFC function should ideally take both stimulus properties into consideration at their design and interpretation stages. While our results suggest that the processing of associativity and valence can vary independently of one another in the mOFC, we also find that valence and associativity are correlated and activate overlapping regions in the mOFC. Therefore, mOFC activity observed while varying stimuli along only one of these dimensions may be partially or even largely, although not necessarily entirely, attributable to the other. More generally, the nature of the relationship between ratings along these two dimensions (at the psychological level) remains a topic of great importance. It may be the case either that positively valenced objects facilitate associative activation and the generation of predictions or that the greater availability of associates for an object makes it perceived more positively or less negatively, in line with the proposal that broader associative activation is linked with better mood (Bar, 2009). Gaining a deeper understanding of how we encode and react to these two stimulus dimensions, and their interaction, will lend importantly to our broader understanding of a more fundamental relationship between factors that have been traditionally dichotomized (and provided a perhaps false appearance of independence) into the domains of cognition and affect.