Skip to main content
Log in

Joint Attention and Early Social Communication: Implications for Research on Intervention with Autism

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Highly structured, intensive early intervention may lead to significant developmental gains for many children with autism. However, a clear understanding of early intervention effects may currently be hampered by a lack of precision in outcome measurement. To improve the precision and sensitivity of outcome assessment it may be useful to integrate research on the nature of the social disturbance of autism with research on early intervention. In this regard, it may be that measures of nonverbal social communication skills are especially important in the study of preschool intervention programs. This is because these measures appear to tap into a cardinal component of the early social disturbance of autism, and because these measures have been directly related to neurological, cognitive, and affective processes that may play a role in autism. The research and theory that support the potential utility of these types of measures for early intervention research are reviewed. Examples are provided to illustrate how these types of measures may assist in addressing current issues and hypotheses about early intervention with autism including the “recovery hypothesis,” the “pivotal skill hypothesis,” and the relative effectiveness of discrete trial versus incidental learning approaches to early intervention. A cybernetic model of autism is also briefly described in an effort to better understand one potential component of early psychoeducational treatment effects with children with autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Adamson, L., & McArthur (1995). Joint attention, affect and culture. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 205–222.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: A review of clinical and experimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32, 627–648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). The eye detection detector and the shared attention mechanism: Two cases for evolutionary psychology. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 41–60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychology, 161, 839–842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birnbrauer, J., & Leach, D. (1993). The Murdoch Early Intervention Program after 2 years. Behavior Change, 10, 63–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1995). Educational approaches in preschool. Behavior techniques in a public school setting. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 311–334). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and interaction in infancy. In J. Call, E. Galenson, & R. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (pp. 38–55). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, R., Chugani, H., Messa, C., Guthrie, D., Sigman, M., Traversay, J., Mundy, P., & Phelps, M. (1993). Hemispherectomy for early onset intractable seizures: Presurgical cerebral glucose metabolism and postsurgical nonverbal communication patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 582–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1981). The communicative intention inventory: A system for observing and coding children's early intentional communication. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 235–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E. (1989). Neuroanatomical systems involved in infantile autism: The implications of cerebral abnormalities. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism (pp. 119–143). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Chisum, H., & Townsend, J. (1994). Neural activity-dependent brain changes in development: Implications for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 697–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 282–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes error: Emotion reason and the human brain. New York: G. P. Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. (1994). Temperament, affective style & frontal lobe asymmetry. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and brain development (pp. 518–536). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G. (1988). Cerebral lateralization in autism: Clues to its role in language and affective development. In S. Segalowitz & D. Molfese (Eds.), Developmental implications of brain lateralization (pp. 437–461). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Galpert, L. (1990). Mother's use of imitative play for facilitating social responsiveness and toy play in young autistic children. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 151–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Klinger, L., Panagiotides, H., Lewy, A., & Castelloe, P. (1995). Subgroups of autistic children based on social behavior display distinct patterns of brain activity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 569–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, attention, and the social-emotional impairments of individuals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism, nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 49–74). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dilavore, P., & Lord, C. (1995). Do you see what I see? Requesting and joint attention in young autistic children. Paper presented at the Conference for the Society of Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

  • Dilavore, P., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (in press). The Prelinguistic Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PL-ADOS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Doehring, P., Benaroya, S., Klaiman, C., & Scuccimarri, C. (1995). Using joint attention, play and imitation skills in the differential diagnosis of young children with autism and with developmental disorders. Paper presented at the Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

  • Fein, D., Pennington, B., & Waterhouse, L. (1987). Implications of social deficits in autism for neurological dysfunction. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism (pp. 127–144). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K., & Rose, S. (1994). Dynamic development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: A framework for theory. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and brain development (pp. 3–66). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, N. (1991). It's not left, it's right: Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 863–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guidetti, M., & Tourrette, C. (1993). Evaluation de la communication sociale precoce-ECSP. Issy-Les-Moulineaux, France: Establissments D'Applications Psychotechniques.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, S., Handleman, J., Gordon, R., Kristoff, B., & Fuentes, F. (1991). Changes in cognitive and language functioning of preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 281–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, P. (1994). Synaptogenesis in the human cerebral cortex. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and brain development (pp. 137–152). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Facilitating early social and communicative development in children with autism. In S. Warren & J. Reichle (Eds.), Perspectives on Communication and Language Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Brooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, L., & Koegel, R. (1995). Motivating communication in children with autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and Cognition in Autism (pp. 73–87). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R., & Frea, W. (1993). Treatment of social behavior in autism through the modification of pivotal social skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 369–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R., Koegel, L., & Schreibman, L. (1991). Assessing and training parents in teaching pivotal behaviors. In R. Prinz (Ed.), Advance in behavioral assessment of families (pp. 65–82). London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, G. (1985). Effects of differences in early social experience on primate neurobiological-behavioral development. In M. Reite & T. Fields (Eds.), The psychobiology of attachment and separation (pp. 135–161). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laski, K., Charlop, M., & Schreibman, L. (1988). Training parents to use the natural language paradigm to increase their autistic children's speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 391–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A., & Happé, E. F. (1989). Autism and ostensive communication: The relevance of metarepresentation. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 205–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewy, A., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention in young autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 555–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEachin, J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long term outcome for children with autism who received early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 359–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, R. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young, autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., & Dunham, P. (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (1993). Normal functioning versus high functioning status in children with autism. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 381–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention, social-emotional approach in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 63–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Nonverbal communication, affective sharing and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 377–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989a). Specifying the nature of the social impairment in autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism (pp. 3–21). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989b). The theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P, Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1993). The theory of mind and joint attention deficits in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 181–203). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in young children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S. (1995). Executive functions in autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 199–220). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., Siving, S., & Normansell, L. (1985). Brain opioids and social emotions. In M. Reite & T. Fields (Eds.), The psychobiology of attachment and separation (pp. 3–49). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. (1996). Brief report: Early intervention in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 243–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scaife, M., & Bruner J. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, E., Short, A., & Mesibov, G. (1989). Relation of behavioral treatment to “normal functioning”: Comment on Lovaas. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 162–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The Early Social-Communication Scales, Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, J., & Oiler, D. K. (1981). Linguistic pragmatics and language intervention strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 75–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheinkopf, S., & Siegel, B. (in press). Home-based treatment of young autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1995). Joint attention across contexts in normal and autistic children. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 189–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Yirmiya, N., & Capps, L. (1995). Social and cognitive understanding in high-functioning children with autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 159–176). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in very young children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, xxx–xxx.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 138–157). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiegerman, E., & Primavera, L. (1984). Imitating the autistic child: Facilitating communicative gaze. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attention in early language development. Language Sciences, 11, 69–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F., Cohen, D., Bregman, J., Hooks, M., & Stevenson, J. (1989). An examination of social typologies in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 82–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. (1986). Ontogeny of communicative functions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 295–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. (1993). Profiling communication and symbolic abilities in young children. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 15, 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 367–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., Yonclas, D., & Bryan, A. (1989). Communication profiles of preschool children with handicaps: Implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 148–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (197S). Social, behavioral, and cognitive characteristics: An epidemiological approach. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 27–46). New York: Plenum Press.

  • Yoder, P., Warren, S., Kim, K., & Gazdag, G. (1994). Facilitating prelinguistic communication in very young children with developmental disabilities II: Systematic replication and extension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 841–851.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilbovicius, M., Garreau, B., Samson, Y., Remy, P., Barthelemy, C., Syrota, A., & Lelord, G. (1995). Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 248–252.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mundy, P., Crowson, M. Joint Attention and Early Social Communication: Implications for Research on Intervention with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 27, 653–676 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025802832021

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025802832021

Keywords

Navigation