Skip to main content
Log in

Preschool Children's Collaborative Problem-Solving Interactions: The Role of Gender, Pair Type, and Task

Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate gender differences in problem-solving and conflict-resolution skills in preschool children. Children between 4 and 5 years of age completed 3 problem-solving tasks with either a same-sex or a different-sex peer. Children's verbal and nonverbal interactions were analyzed. Girls used mitigation more often than did boys. Mixed-sex dyads engaged in controlling verbal interactions more often than same-sex dyads. There were relationships between verbal and nonverbal behaviors and task success; these relationships also differed across pair types. The results of the study demonstrate that the gender differences in types of verbal interactions previously observed in preschool children's free play are also present in their problem-solving interactions and that children are able to alter the types of behaviors they use depending upon both partner gender and the type of task involved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ashley, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development, 7, 143-163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausch, L. (1994). Gender comparisons of young children's social interaction in cooperative play activity. Sex Roles, 31, 225-239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem solving: When are two heads better than one? Child Development, 59, 87-96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azmitia, M. (1996). Peer interactive minds: Developmental, theoretical, and methodological issues. In P. B. Baltes & V. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds:Life-span perspectives on the social foundations of cognition (pp. 133-162). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azmitia, M., & Perlmutter, M. (1989). Social influences on children's cognition: State of the art and future directions. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 22, pp. 89-144). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbieri, M. S., & Light, P. H. (1992). Interaction, gender, and performance on a computer-based problem solving task. Learning and Instruction, 2, 199-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bearison, D. J., Magzamen, S., & Filardo, E. (1986). Socio-cognitive conflict and cognitive growth in young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 32, 51-72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C. A., & Carriger, M. S. (1991). Collaborations among toddler peers: Individual contributions to social contexts. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 365-383). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannella, G. S. (1992). Gender composition and conflict in dyadic sociocognitive interaction: Effects on spatial learning in young children. Journal of Experimental Education, 6, 29-41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannella, G. S. (1993). Learning through social interaction: Shared cognitive experience, negotiation strategies, and joint concept construction for young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 427-444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannella, G. S., Viruru, R., & Amin, S. (1995). Sociocognitive growth and the young child: Comparisons with spatial and literacy content. Child Study Journal, 25, 213-231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L., & Bukatko, D. (2000). Gender, communication, and social influence: A developmental perspective. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 295-331). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlesworth, W. R., & Dzur, C. (1987). Gender comparisons of preschoolers' behavior and resource utilization in group problem solving. Child Development, 58, 191-200.

    Google Scholar 

  • d'Estrėe T. P., & Babbitt, E. F. (1998). Women and the art of peacemaking: Data from Israeli-Palestinian interactive problem-solving workshops. Political Psychology, 19, 185-209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duran, R. T., & Gauvain, M. (1993). The role of age versus expertise in peer collaboration during joint planning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 227-242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, A. R., & Garvey, C. (1981). Children's use of verbal strategies in resolving conflicts. Discourse Processes, 4, 149-170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S., Schneiders, B., & Smith, H. D. (1991, June). Children's collaborative problem solving: Effects of expertise in the context of relationships or can friends be considered "experts?" Paper presented at the annual symposium of the Jean Piaget Society, Philadelphia.

  • Fishman, P. M. (1983). Interaction: The work women do. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender, and society (pp. 89-101). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goncu, A., & Kessel, F. (1988). Preschoolers' collaborative construction in planning and maintaining imaginative play. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 327-344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H. (1988). Cooperation and competition across girls' play activities. In A. D. Todd & S. Fisher (Eds.), Gender and discourse: The power of talk (pp. 55-92). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W., French, D. C., Laursen, B., Johnston, M. K., & Ogawa, J. R. (1993). Conflict and friendship relations in middle childhood: Behavior in a closed-field situation. Child Development, 64, 445-454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M., Brackenridge, A., Bibby, A., & Greenough, P. (1988). Girls, boys, and turtles: Gender effects in young children learning with Logo. In C. Hoyles (Ed.), Girls and computers (pp. 31-39). London: Institute of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1978). Social behavior at thirty-three months in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557-569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keashly, L. (1994). Gender and conflict: What does psychological research tell us? In A. Taylor & J. B. Miller (Eds.), Conflict and gender (pp. 167-190). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen, M., & Naigles, L. (1995). Preschool children pay attention to their addressees: Effects of gender composition on peer disputes. Discourse Processes, 19, 329-346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, A., Yebra, M., & Willis, M. (1995, March). Task type affects styles of peer interaction. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

  • Kyratzis, A. (1992). Gender differences in the use of persuasive justification in children's pretend play. In K. Hall, M. Bucholtz, & B. Moonwomon (Eds.), Locating power: Proceedings of the second Berkeley Women and Language Conference (pp. 326-337). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1991). Influence and involvement in children's discourse: Age, gender, and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797-811.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1994). Exploring the consequences of gender segregation on social relationships. In C. Leaper (Ed.), New directions for child development (pp. 67-86). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Littleton, K., Light, P., Joiner, R., Messer, D., & Barnes, P. (1992). Pairing and gender effects on children's computer-based learning. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 7, 311-324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madhok, J. J. (1992). The effect of gender composition on group interaction. In K. Hall, M. Bucholtz, & B. Moonwomon (Eds.), Locating power: Proceedings of the Second Berkeley Women and Language Conference (pp. 371-385). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. M., Danaher, D. L., & Forbes, D. (1986). Sex-related strategies for coping with interpersonal conflict in children aged five and seven. Developmental Psychology, 22, 543-548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mugny, G., & Doise, W. (1978). Socio-cognitive conflict and structure of individual and collective performances. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 181-192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulac, A., Baradac, J. J., & Gibbons, P. (2001). Empirical support for the gender-as-culture hypothesis: An intercultural analysis of male/female language differences. Human Communications Research, 27, 121-152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, A. A., & Perlmutter, M. (1985). Preschool children's problem-solving interactions at computers and jigsaw puzzles. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 173-186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, M., Behrend, S. D., Kuo, F., & Muller, A. (1989). Social influences on children's problem solving. Developmental Psychology, 25, 744-754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E., & Damon, W. (1989). Problem solving with equals: Peer collaboration as a context for learning mathematics and spatial concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 639-646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pozzi, S., Healy, L., & Hoyles, C. (1993). Learning and interaction in groups with computers: When do ability and gender matter? Social Development, 2, 22-241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, T. L., & Schneer, J. A. (1994). Gender differences in conflict-handling styles: Less than meets the eye? In A. Taylor & J. B. Miller (Eds.), Conflict and gender (pp. 167-190). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Sprafkin, C., Elman, M., & Doyle, A. (1982). The early development of sex-differentiated patterns of influence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 14, 351-363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1990). Pickle fights: Gendered talk in preschool disputes. Discourse Processes, 13, 5-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495-552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., Jindal, N., & Underwood, J. (1994). Gender differences and effects of co-operation in a computer-based task. Educational Research, 36, 63-74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., McCaffrey, M., & Underwood, J. (1990). Gender differences in a cooperative computer-based task. Educational Research, 32, 44-49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verba, M. (1994). The beginnings of collaboration in peer interaction. Human Development, 37, 125-139.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heather A. Holmes-Lonergan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Holmes-Lonergan, H.A. Preschool Children's Collaborative Problem-Solving Interactions: The Role of Gender, Pair Type, and Task. Sex Roles 48, 505–517 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023523228455

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023523228455

Navigation