Skip to main content
Log in

Meta-Analyses of Gender Effects on Conversational Interruption: Who, What, When, Where, and How

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Meta-analyses of 43 published studies comparingadult women's and men's interruptions duringconversations were conducted. Combined significancelevels and combined effect sizes were analyzed. Acrossstudies, men were significantly more likely than womento use interruptions. This difference, however, wasassociated with a negligible effect size (d = .15). Amore substantial effect size (d = .33) was found when studies looking specifically at intrusive typesof interruption were analyzed separately. Othermoderator variables were found to be related to gendereffects on the use of intrusive interruptions. Most notably, reports of gender differences inintrusive interruptions were more likely and larger inmagnitude when either women (versus men) were firstauthors, participants were observed in naturalistic(versus laboratory) settings, or participants wereobserved interacting in groups of three or more persons(versus in dyads). These results lend support to acontextual-interactive model of gender that emphasizes the importance of situational moderators ongender-related variations in social behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction: Reconsidering the difference. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (1995). Analyzing interaction: Sequential analysis with SDIS and GSEQ. New York: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Observing interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beall, A. E. (1993). A social constructionist view of gender. In A. E. Beall & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beattie, G. (1977). The dynamics of interruption and the filled pause. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 283–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Beattie, G. W. (1981). Interruption in conversational interaction and its relation to the sex and status of the interactants. Linguistics, 19, 5–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bilous, F. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1988). Dominance and accommodation in the conversational behaviors of same-and mixed-gender dyads. Language and Communication, 8, 183–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bohn, E., & Stutman, R. (1983). Sex role differences in the relational control dimension of dyadic interaction. Women's Studies in Communication, 6, 965–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Brooks, V. R. (1982). Sex differences in student dominance behavior in female and male professors' classrooms. Sex Roles, 8, 683–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, K. E., Kleim, D. M., & Olson, K. R. (1992). Conversational activity and interruptions among men and women. The Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 419–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Carli, L. L. (1990). Gender, language, and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 941–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Case, S. S. (1988). Cultural differences, not deficiencies: An analysis of managerial women's language. In S. Rose & L. Larwood (Eds.), Women's careers: Pathways and pitfalls. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambliss, C. A., & Feeny, N. (1992). Effect of sex of subject, sex of interrupter, and topic of conversation on the perceptions of interruptions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1235–1241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Craig, D., & Pitts, M. K. (1990). The dynamics of dominance in tutorial discussion. Linguistics, 28, 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, M. (1995). Talking difference: On gender and language. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Ruback, R. B. (1984). Vocal patterns in male and female groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 518–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • *De Boer, M. (1987). Sex differences in language: Observations of dyadic conversations between members of the same sex. In D. Brouwer & D. de Haan (Eds.), Women's language, socialization and self-image. Providence, RI: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Dindia, K. (1987). The effects of sex of subject and sex of partner on interruptions. Human Communication Research, 13, 345–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drass, K. A. (1986). The effect of gender identity on conversation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 49, 294–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed communications as determinants of sex differences in influenceability: A meta-analysis of social influence studies. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Eakins, B., & Eakins, R. G. (1976). Verbal turn-taking and exchanges in faculty dialogue. In B. L. Dubois & I. M. Crouch (Eds.), The sociology of the languages of American women. San Antonio, TX: Trinity University.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Fallon, J., & Guo, D. (1994). The relationship between topic familiarity and conversational dominance. Psychology, a Journal of Human Behavior, 31, 53–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, N. (1977). Simultaneous speech, interruptions and dominance. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16, 295–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Frances, S. J. (1979). Sex differences in nonverbal behavior. Sex Roles, 5, 519–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, J. A. (1990). Interrupting the discourse on interruptions: An analysis in terms of relationally neutral, power-and rapport-oriented acts. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 883–903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, K. (1991). Some consequences of deep interruption in task-oriented communication. Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 10, 185–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, D., & Clarke, S. (1992). Interruptions, gender, and power: A critical review of the literature. In K. Hall, M. Bucholtz, & B. Moonwoman (Eds.), Locating power: Proceedings of the Second Berkeley Women and Language Conference. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, D., & Clarke, S. (19930). Women, men, and interruptions: A critical review. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Gender and conversational interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Johnson, C. (1994). Gender, legitimate authority, and leader-subordinate conversations. American Sociological Review, 59, 122–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Jones, E. S., & Gallois, C., Callan, V. J., & Barker, M. (1995). Language and power in an academic context: The effects of status, ethnicity, and sex. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 434–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kennedy, C. W., & Camden, C. (1983). A new look at interruptions. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 47, 45–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kollock, P., Blumstein, P., & Schwartz, P. (1985). Sex and power in interaction: Conversational privileges and duties. American Sociological Review, 50, 34–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • *LaFrance, M., & Carmen, B. (1980). The nonverbal display of psychological androgyny. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 38, 36–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1994). Exploring the consequences of gender segregation on social relationships. In C. Leaper (Ed.), Childhood gender segregation: Causes and consequences (New Directions for child Development, No. 65). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Anderson, K. J. (1997). Gender development and heterosexual romantic relationships during adolescence. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & S. Shulman & W. A. Collins (Issue Eds.), Romantic relationships in adolescence: Developmental perspectives (New Directions for Child Development, No. 78). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 3–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Leet-Pellegrini, H. M. (1980). Conversational dominance as a function of gender and expertise. In H. Giles, W. P. Robinson, & P. M. Smith (Eds.), Language: Social psychological perspectives. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Leffler, A., Gillespie, D., & Conaty, J. C. (1982). The effects of status differentiation on nonverbal behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Makri-Tsilipakou, M. (1994). Interruption revisited: Affiliative vs. disaffiliative intervention. Journal of Pragmatics, 21, 401–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Marche, T. A., & Peterson, C. (1993). The development and sex-related use of interruption behavior. Human Communication Research, 19, 388–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Margolin, G., & Wampold, B. E. (1981). Sequential analysis of conflict and accord in distressed and nondistressed marital partners. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 49, 554–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Martin, J. N., & Craig, R. T. (1983). Selected linguistic sex differences during initial social interactions of same-sex and mixed-sex student dyads. The Western Journal of Speech Communication, 47, 16–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • *McLachlan, A. (1991). The effects of agreement, disagreement, gender and familiarity on patterns of dyadic interaction. Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 10, 199–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, M. L. (1984). Conversation: How talk is organized. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • *McMillan, J. R., Clifton, A. K., McGrath, D., & Gale, W. S. (1977). Women's language: Uncertainty, or interpersonal sensitivity and emotionality? Sex Roles, 3, 545–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mott, H., & Petrie, H. (1995). Workplace interactions: Women's linguistic behavior. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 324–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murata, K. (1994). Intrusive or co-operative? A cross-cultural study of interruption. Journal of Pragmatics, 21, 385–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, S. O., & Covelli, L. H. (1988). Women and men speaking at the same time. Journal of Pragmatics, 12, 103–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Natale, M., Entin, E., & Jaffe, J. (1979). Vocal interruptions in dyadic communication as a function of speech and social anxiety. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 37, 865–878.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Nemeth, C., Endicott, J., & Wachtler, J. (1976). From the '50s to the '70s: Women in jury deliberations. Sociometry, 39, 293–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ng, S. H., Brook, M., & Dunne, M. (1995). Interruption and influence in discussion groups. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 369–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Nohara, M. (1992). Sex differences in interruption. An experimental reevaluation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 127–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Redeker, G., & Maes, A. (1996). Gender differences in interruptions. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rim, Y. (1977). Personality variables and interruptions in small discussion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 7, 247–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, L. F., & Reis, H. T. (1989). The effects on interruption, gender and status on interpersonal perceptions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 141–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Roger, D., Nesshoever, W. (1987). Individual differences in dyadic conversational strategies: A further study. British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 247–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Roger, D. B., & Schumacher, A. (1983). Effects of individual differences on dyadic conversational strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 700–705.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rogers, W. T., & Jones, S. E. (1980). Effects of dominance tendencies on floor holding and interruption behavior in dyadic interaction. Human Communication Research, 1, 113–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Shaw, M. E., & Sadler, O. W. (1965). Interaction patterns in heterosexual dyads varying in degree of intimacy. Journal of Social Psychology, 66, 345–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Simkins-Bullock, J., & Wildman, B. G. (1991). An investigation into the relationships between gender and language. Sex Roles, 24, 149–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Smeltzer, L. R., & Watson, K. W. (1986). Gender differences in verbal communication during negotiations. Communication Research Reports, 3, 74–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Smith, H. W. (1977). Small group interaction at various ages: Simultaneous talking and interruptions of others. Small Group Behavior, 8, 65–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith-Lovin, L., & Brody, C. (1989). Interruptions in group discussions: The effects of gender and group composition. American Sociological Review, 54, 424–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Street, R. L., Jr., & Murphy, T. L. (1987). Interpersonal orientation and speech behavior. Communication Monographs, 54, 42–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1983). When is an overlap not an interruption? One component of conversational style. In R. J. DePietro, W. Frawley, & A. Wedel (Eds.), The first Delaware Symposium on Language Studies. Newark, DE: University of Delaware Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1994). Interpreting interruption in conversation. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Gender and discourse. New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thimm, C., Rademacher, U., & Kruse, L. (1995). “Power-related talk”: Control in verbal interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 382–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. P., Roger, D., & Bull, P. (1983). A sequential analysis of informal dyadic conversation using Markov chains. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Trimboli, C., & Walker, M. B. (1984). Switching pauses in cooperative and competitive conversations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 297–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Welkowitz, J., Bond, R. N., & Feldstein, S. (1984). Conversational time patterns of Japanese-American adults and children in same and mixed-gender dyads. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 3, 127–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • *West, C., & Zimmerman, D. (1983). Small insults: A study of interruptions in cross-sec conversations between unacquainted person. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, F. N., & Williams, S. J. (1976). Simultaneous talking in conversation and sex of speakers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 1067–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Karten, S. J. (1986). Sex differences in interaction style as a product of perceived sex differences in competence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 50, 341–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, N. (1989). Talking shop: Sex and status as determinants of floor apportionment in a work setting. In J. Coates & D. Cameron (Eds.), Women in their speech communities. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D. H., & West, C. (1975). Sex roles, interruptions and silences in conversation. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and sex: Difference and dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Anderson, K.J., Leaper, C. Meta-Analyses of Gender Effects on Conversational Interruption: Who, What, When, Where, and How. Sex Roles 39, 225–252 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018802521676

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018802521676

Keywords

Navigation