Skip to main content
Log in

The Epigenesis of Meaning in Human Beings, and Possibly in Robots

  • Published:
Minds and Machines Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article addresses a classical question: Can a machine use language meaningfully and if so, how can this be achieved? The first part of the paper is mainly philosophical. Since meaning implies intentionality on the part of the language user, artificial systems which obviously lack intentionality will be `meaningless' (pace e.g. Dennett). There is, however, no good reason to assume that intentionality is an exclusively biological property (pace e.g. Searle) and thus a robot with bodily structures, interaction patterns and development similar to those of human beings would constitute a system possibly capable of meaning – a conjecture supported through a Wittgenstein-inspired thought experiment. The second part of the paper focuses on the empirical and constructive questions. Departing from the principle of epigenesis stating that during every state of development new structure arises on the basis of existing structure plus various sorts of interaction, a model of human cognitive and linguistic development is proposed according to which physical, social and linguistic interactions between the individual and the environment have their respective peaks in three consecutive stages of development: episodic, mimetic and symbolic. The transitions between these stages are qualitative, and bear a similarity to the stages in phylogenesis proposed by Donald (1991) and Deacon (1997). Following the principle of epigenetic development, robotogenesis could possibly recapitulate ontogenesis, leading to the emergence of intentionality, consciousness and meaning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baldwin, J. (1913), Social and Ethical Interpretations in Mental Development. A Study in Social Psychology, 5th ed. New York: MacMillan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balkenius, C. (1995), Natural Intelligence in Artificial Creatures, Lund University Cognitive Studies 37.

  • Balkenius, C. (forthcoming), ‘Attention, Habituation and Conditioning: Towards a Computational Model’, Cognitive Science Quarterly 1.

  • Bates, E. (1979), The Emergence of Symbols. Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, B. (in press), ‘On the Nature and Nurture of Language’, in E. Bizzi, P. Calissano, and V. Volterra, eds., Frontiere della Biologia The Brain of Homo sapiens, Rome: Giovanni Trecani.

  • Bateson, M. (1975), ‘Mother-infant Exchanges: The Epigenesis of Conversational interaction’ in D. Aaronson and R. Rieber, eds., Developmental Psycholinguistics and Communication Disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 263). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (1993), ‘Language Development’, Handbook of Psycholinguistics, New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., Breazeal, C., Marjanovic, M, Scassellati, B. and Williamson, M. (1999), ‘The Cog Project: Building a Humanoid Robot’, in C.L. Nehaniv, ed., Computation for Metaphors, Analogy, and Agents, Springer-Verlag Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 1562.

  • Bruner, J. (1983), Child's Talk, New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986), Actual Minds, Possible Worlds, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buford, T. (1971), Essays on Other Minds, University of Illinois Press.

  • Butterworth, G. (1991), ‘The Ontogeny and Philogeny of Joint Visual Attention’, in A. Whiten, ed., Natural Theories of Mind. London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1995), ‘An Ecological Perspective on the Origins of the Self’ in J.L. Bermudez, A. Marcel, and N. Eilan, eds., The Body and the Self, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (1996), Language, Thought and Consciousness, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (1998), Mind and Language 13(4), 457–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J. (1985), Neuronal Man, New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980), Rules and Representations, New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P.M. and Churchland, P.S. (1990), ‘Could a Machine Think?’ Scientific American, January 1990: 26–31.

  • Clark, H.H. and Marshall, C.R. (1981) ‘Definite Reference and Mutual Knowledge’, in Joshi, Webber and Sag, eds., Elements of Discourse Understanding, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtiss, S. (1977), Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-day “Wild Child”. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997), The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain, New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1987), The Intentional Stance, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1991), Consciousness Explained, Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1995), ‘Cog: Steps Towards Consciousness in Robots’ in T. Metzinger, ed., Conscious Erperience. Lawrence, Kansas: Allen Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1996), Kinds of Minds. Towards an Understanding of Consciousness, London: Phenix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991), Origins of the Modern Mind. Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drescher, G. (1991), Made-up Minds. A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (1991), Being-in-the-world. A Commentary on Heidegger's “Being and Time, Division I”, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (1993) [1972], What Computers (Still) Can't Do. A Critique of Artificial Reason, Third revised edition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1996), Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language, London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. (1989), The Remembered Present. A Biological Theory of Consciousness. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. (1992), Bright Air, Brilliant Fire. On the Matter of the Mind, New York: Basic Books, HarperCollins Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Bates, E., Thal, D. and Pethick, S.J. (1994), ‘Variability in Early Communicative Development’. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Developjnent, Serial No. 242. Vol. 59, No. 5.

  • Firth, U. (1989), ‘A New Look at Language and Communication in Autism’, British Journal of Disorders of Communication 24, 123–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A. (1983), The Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. (1970), ‘Chimpanzees: Self recognition’, Science 167, 86–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J. (1979), The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J. (1994), ‘Mutual Awareness in Primate Communication: A Gricean approach’, in S. Parker, R. Mitchel and M. Boccia, eds., Self-awareness in Animals and Humans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik. A., Choi, S. and Baumberger, T. (1996), ‘Cross-linguistic Differences in Early Semantic and Cognitive Development’. Cognitive Development 11(2), 197–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, P. (1957), ‘Meaning’, Philosophical Review 66, 377–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2000), Conceptual Spaces. The Geometry of Thought, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haeckel, E. (1874), The Evolution of Man: A Popular Exposition of the Principle Points of Human Ontogeny and Phylogeny, New York: International Science Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1991), ‘Other Bodies, Other Minds: A Machine Incarnation of an Old Philosophical Problem’, Minds and Machines 1, 43–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1993). ‘Grounding Symbols in the Analog World with Neural Nets’, Think 2(1), 12–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. (1991), ‘The Work of Imagination’ in A. Whiten, ed., Natural Theories of Mind. London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, C. (1951), The Ape in Our House, New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1955), ‘Drive and the C.N.S.’ Psychological Review 62, 243–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, D. (1989), First Language Acquisition. Method, Description, and Explanation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itkonen, E. (1983), Causality in Linguistic Theory. A Critical Investigation into the Philosophical and Methodological Foundations of ‘Non-autonomous’ Linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itkonen, E. (1997), ‘The Social Ontology of Meaning’, SKY 1997 Yearbook of the Linguistic Association of Finland, 49–80.

  • Jusezyk, P.W. (1997), The Discovery of Spoken Language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozima, H. (1999), ‘Attention-Sharing, Behavior-Sharing and the Acquisition of Language’. Paper presented at the international symposium on “The ecology of language acquisition”, January 1999, Amsterdam.

  • Leder, D. (1990), The Absent Body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenat, D. and Feigenbaum, E. (1991), ‘On the Thresholds of Knowledge’, Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3).

  • Leneberg, E. (1980), ‘Of Language Knowledge, Apes and Brains’ in T. Sebeok and J. Sebeok, eds., ‘Speaking of Apes’. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. and J. Brooks-Gunn (1979), Social Cognition and the Acquisition of the Self. New York: Plenum. Liège, August 11–15, 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1993), ‘Phases in the Child's Development of Language’, American Scientist 82, 436–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malcolm, N. (1971), ‘Knowledge of Other Minds’, in T. Buford, ed., Essays on Other Minds. University of Illinois Press.

  • Mead, G. (1934), Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M. (1977), ‘Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates’, Science 198, 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M. (1995), ‘Infants' Understanding of People and Things: From Body Imitation to Folk Psychology’, in J.L. Bermudez, A. Marcel and N. Eilan, eds., The Body and the Self. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, R. (1996), ‘Innateness, Autonomy, Universality? Neurological Approaches to Language’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 611–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1988), ‘Five Kinds of Self-knowledge’, Philosophical Psychology 1, 35–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1995). ‘The Self Perceived’, in U. Neisser, ed., The Perceived Self. Ecological and lnterpersonal Sources of Self knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1953), The Origin of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, C.S. (1931–35), The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Vols. 1–4. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994), The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. (1976), Intelligence in Ape and Man. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1960), Minds and Machines, in S. Hook, ed., Dimensions of Mind. New York: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, W. (1988), ‘Syntactic Semantics: Foundations of computational natural language understanding’, in H. Fetzer, ed., Aspects of Artificial lntelligence. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regier, T. (1996), The Human Semantic Potential: Spatial Language and Constrained Connectionism. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, T. (1912) [1785], ‘Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man’, in B. Rand, ed., The Classical Psychologists. Cambridge: Riverside Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M. (1998), ‘Language within our Grasp’, Trends in Neurosciences 21, 188–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949), The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson's University Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F. de (1916), Cours de Linguistigue Générale. Paris: Payot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. and Taylor, T. (1998), Apes, Language and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scassellati, B. (in press), ‘Investigating Models of Social Development Using a Humanoid Robot’, Presented at the 1998 AAAI Fall Symposium “Robots and Biology: Developing Connections” in Orlando, Florida.

  • Schoenemann, P. (1999), ‘Syntax as an Emergent Characteristic of the Evolution of Semantic Complexity’, Minds and Machines 9(3), 309–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1980), ‘Minds, Brains and Programs’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3, 417–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1995), ‘The Mystery of Consciousness’, The New York Review of Books, November 2, 1995: 61–66.

  • Tomasello, M. (1995), ‘On the Interpersonal Origins of the Self-concept’, in U. Neisser, ed., The Perceived Self. Ecological and interpersonal Sources of Self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999), The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kdruger, A. and Ratner, H. (1993), ‘Cultural Learning’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(3), 495–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985), ‘How Many Memory Systems are There?’, American Psychologist, April 1985: 385–398.

  • Turing, A. (1950), ‘Computing Machinery and Intelliegnce’, Mind 59, 433–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1997), ‘Anticipation and the Constructivist Theory of Cognition’, Presented at International Conference on Computing Anticinatory Systems.

  • von Hofsten, C. (1989), ‘Transition Mechanisms in Sensorimotor Development’, in A. de Ribaupierre, ed., Transition Mechanisms in Child Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978), Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1986) [1934], Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953), Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1969), The Blue and Brown Books. London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J. (1997), Situated Embodiment. Studies in the Emergence of Spatial Meaning. Ph.D. Thesis. Stockholm University, Stockholm: Gotab.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J. (2000), ‘The Mimetic Origins of Self-Conciousness in phylo-, onto-and robotogenesis’, Paper presented at Third Asia-Pacific Conference on Simulated Evolution and Learning (SEAL2000), Nagoya, Japan.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zlatev, J. The Epigenesis of Meaning in Human Beings, and Possibly in Robots. Minds and Machines 11, 155–195 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011218919464

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011218919464

Navigation