Skip to main content
Log in

Embodied cognition as grounding for situatedness and context in mathematics education

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper we analyze, from the perspective of 'Embodied Cognition', why learning and cognition are situated and context-dependent. We argue that the nature of situated learning and cognition cannot be fully understood by focusing only on social, cultural and contextual factors. These factors are themselves further situated and made comprehensible by the shared biology and fundamental bodily experiences of human beings. Thus cognition itself is embodied, and the bodily-grounded nature of cognition provides a foundation for social situatedness, entails a reconceptualization of cognition and mathematics itself, and has important consequences for mathematics education. After framing some theoretical notions of embodied cognition in the perspective of modern cognitive science, we analyze a case study – continuity of functions. We use conceptual metaphor theory to show how embodied cognition, while providing grounding for situatedness, also gives fruitful results in analyzing the cognitive difficulties underlying the understanding of continuity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Brooks, R. and Stein, L. A.: 1993, Building Brains for Bodies, A.I. Memo No. 1439. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.

  • Cobb, P.: 1994, 'Where is the mind? Constructivist and sociocultural perspectives on mathematical development', Educational Researcher 23(7), 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Yackel, E. and Wood, T.: 1992, 'A constructivist alternative to the representational view of mind', Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 23(1), 2–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A., Brown, J.S. and Newman, S.: 1989, 'Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching students the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics', in L.B. Resnick (ed.), Knowing, Learning, and Instruction: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 453–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J.: 1990, 'A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics, science, and programming', in C. Cazden (ed.), Review of Research in Education, 16, American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC, pp. 3–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J.: 1995, 'A theory of intellectual development', For the Learning of Mathematics 15(1), 38–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csordas, T.J.: 1994, Embodiment and Experience: The Existential Ground of Culture and Self, University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiSessa, A, 1983, 'Phenomenology and the evolution of intuition', in D. Gentner and A. Stevens (eds.), Mental Models, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 15–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G.: 1992, Bright Air, Brilliant Fire, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. and Núñez, R.: 1995, 'Cognitive science and mathematics education: A non-objectivist view', Proceedings of the 19th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education, PME. Vol. 2, pp. 240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenk, M. and Keane, M.: 1992, Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Manual, Erlbaum, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J., Lakoff, G., Bailey, D., Narayanan, S., Regier, T. and Stolcke, A.: 1996, 'L0 — The first five years of an automated language acquisition project', Artificial Intelligence Review 10(1).

  • Gardner, H.: 1985, The Mind's New Science, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J.: 1997, 'On claims that answer the wrong questions', Educational Researcher, 26, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M.: 1987, The Body in the Mind, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P.: 1997, April, Personal communication, University of California at San Diego.

  • Lakoff, G.: 1987, Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G.: 1993, 'The contemporary theory of metaphor', in A. Ortony (ed.) Metaphor and Thought, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 202–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.: 1980, Metaphors we Live by, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.: 1998, Philosophy in the Flesh, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R., 1997, 'The metaphorical structure of mathematics: Sketching out cognitive foundations for a mind-based mathematics', in L. English (ed.), Mathematical Reasoning: Analogies, Metaphors, and Images, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 21–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R.: forthcoming, Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Creates Mathematics.

  • Lave, J.: 1988, Cognition in Practice: Mind, Mathematics and Culture in Everyday Life, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. and Wenger, E.: 1991, Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lock, M.: 1993, 'Cultivating the body: Anthropology and epistemologies of bodily practice and knowledge', Annual Review of Anthropology 22, 133–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. and Varela, F.: 1987, The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding, New Science Library, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M.: 1945, La Phénoménologie de la Perception, Gallimard, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T. and Bryant, P.: 1996, Children Doing Mathematics, Blackwells, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R.: 1993, En deçà du transfini: Aspects psychocognitifs sous-jacents au concept d'infini en mathématiques, University Press, Fribourg, Switzerland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R.: 1995, 'What brain for God's-eye? Biological naturalism, ontological objectivism, and searle', Journal of Consciousness Studies 2(2), 149–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R.: 1997, 'Eating soup with chopsticks: Dogmas, difficulties, and alternatives in the study of conscious experience', Journal of Consciousness Studies 4(2), 143–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. and Lakoff, G.: 1998, 'What did weierstrass really define? The cognitive structure of natural and εδ continuity', Mathematical Cognition 4(2), 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierpont, J.: 1899, 'On the arithmetization of mathematics', Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 394–406.

  • Regier, T.: 1996, The Human Semantic Potential, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robert, A.: 1982, 'L'acquisition de la notion de convergence de suites numériques dans l'enseignement supérieur', Recherches en didactique des mathématiques, 3, 307–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B.: 1990, Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. and Lave, J. (eds.): 1984, Everyday Cognition: Its Development in Social Context, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E.: 1973, 'Natural categories', Cognitive Psychology 4, 328–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., 1994, 'Categorization', in V.S. Ramachandran (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, Academic Press, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanford, A.: 1985, Cognition and Cognitive Psychology, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, G.F.: 1985, Calculus with Analytic Geometry, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J.P, diSessa, A. and Roschelle, J.: 1993/94, 'Misconceptions reconceived: A constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition', Journal of the Learning Sciences 3(2), 115–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tall, D.O. and Vinner, S.: 1981, 'Concept image and concept definition in mathematics with particular reference to limits and continuity', Educational Studies in Mathematics 12, 151–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L.: 1988, 'Force dynamics in language and cognition', Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. and Smith, L.: 1994, A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E. and Rosch, E.: 1991, The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Glasersfeld, E.: 1990, 'An exposition of constructivism: Why some like it radical', in R. Davis, C. Maher and N. Noddings (eds.), Constructivist Views on the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Reston, VA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walkerdine, V.: 1982, 'From context to text: A psychosemiotic approach to abstract thought', in M. Beveridge (ed.), Children Thinking through Language, Edward Arnold, London.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Núñez, R.E., Edwards, L.D. & Filipe Matos, J. Embodied cognition as grounding for situatedness and context in mathematics education. Educational Studies in Mathematics 39, 45–65 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003759711966

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003759711966

Keywords

Navigation