Skip to main content
Log in

Aversive Events as Positive Reinforcers: An Investigation of Avoidance and Safety Signals in Humans

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Introduction

It is well established that stimuli correlated with the absence of aversive events may come to signal periods of safety from those events. In previous research, stimuli that signaled periods of safety were largely neutral in nature. In the present investigation, we examined whether aversive events also could be conditioned as safety signals.

Methods

Seven university students were initially exposed to a 72 dB tone, which all rated as highly aversive. Next, they were required to play a computerized game in which mouse clicks on a map produced points (“treasures”) or point losses (“bombs”). Participants were instructed that bombs could be disabled by pressing a foot pedal, although pedal presses produced the aversive tone. In the next phase of the game, no bombs were programmed. During some sessions, pedal presses produced the tone and in other sessions they did not.

Results

Rates of pedal pressing maintained when the sound was produced; however, pedal pressing eventually ceased in sessions in which it was absent. Follow-up measures indicated that 6 of the 7 participants rated the tone as aversive at the end of the study.

Discussion

These findings suggest that the aversive stimulus functioned as a positive reinforcer, given that participants chose to produce the sound even when it did not abate aversive stimulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 380–447). New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bizo, L. A., Remington, B., D’Souza, L. S., Heighway, S. K., & Baston, C. (2002). Human variable ratio performance. Learning and Motivation, 33, 411–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burchardt, C. J., & Levis, D. J. (1977). Utility of presenting slides of a phobic stimulus in context of a behavioral avoidance procedure. Behavior Therapy, 8, 340–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Candido, A., Gonzalez, F., & de Brugada, I. (2004). Safety signals from avoidance learning but not from yoked classical conditioning training pass both summation and retardation tests for inhibition. Behavioural Processes, 66, 153–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciminero, A. R., & Davidson, R. S. (1977). Free-operant avoidance of alcohol: Analog study of alcohol aversion. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 8, 371–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, M. R., Jou, R. L., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2009). Avoiding negative outcomes: Tracking the mechanisms of avoidance learning in humans during fear conditioning. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 3, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1954). Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review, 61, 34–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1977). Escape, avoidance, punishment: Where do we stand? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28, 83–95.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001a). Still no evidence for temporally extended shock-frequency reduction as a reinforcer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 367–378.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001b). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 311–333.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A., & Sears, G. W. (1973). Control of avoidance by a response-produced stimulus. Learning and Motivation, 4, 284–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmelkamp, P. M., Kraaijkamp, H. J., & van den Hout, M. A. (1999). Assessment of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behavior Modification, 23, 269–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernando, A., Urcelay, G., Mar, A., Dickinson, A., & Robbins, T. (2014). Free-operant avoidance behaviour by rats after reinforcer revaluation using opioid agonists and D amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 6286–6293.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485–496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fullana, M. A., Tortella-Feliu, M., Caseras, X., Andion, O., Torrubia, R., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2005). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the obsessive-compulsive inventory–revised in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 893–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galizio, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 53–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., Huppert, J. D., Simons, R. F., & Foa, E. B. (2004). Psychometric properties of the OCI-R in a college sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 115–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, R. J., & Rachman, S. (1977). Obsessional-compulsive complaints. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 389–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., & Metalsky, G. I. (2001). Excessive reassurance seeking: Delineating a risk factor involved in the development of depressive symptoms. Psychological Science, 12, 371–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, P. J. (2006). Is avoiding an aversive outcome rewarding? Neural substrates of avoidance learning in the human brain. PLoS Biology, 4, 1453–1461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing a technology for the use of operant extinction in clinical settings: An examination of basic and applied research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 345–385.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1951). Two-factor learning theory: Summary and comment. Psychological Review, 58, 350–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 385–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Establishment of a positive reinforcer through contrast with shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 260–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: A cognitive account. Behavioural Psychotherapy, Special Issue: The Changing Face of Behavioural Psychotherapy, 19, 6–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance. In P. H. Hoch & J. Zubi (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 70–99). New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1989). Avoidance at Columbia. Behavior Analyst, 12, 191–195.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, P., & Weinberger, J. (2009). Very brief exposure: The effects of unrepeatable stimuli on fearful behavior. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 939–951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberger, L. G., & Burns, G. L. (1990). Compulsive activity checklist and the Maudsley obsessional-compulsive inventory: Psychometric properties of two measures of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 21, 117–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taffou, M., Guerchouche, R., Drettakis, G., & Viaud-Delmon, I. (2013). Auditory-visual aversive stimuli modulate the conscious experience of fear. Multisensory Research, 26, 347–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unger, W., Evans, I. M., Rourke, P., & Levis, D. J. (2003). The S-S construct of expectancy versus S-R construct of fear: Which motivates the acquisition of avoidance behavior? Journal of General Psychology, 2, 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Matilda Angelaki for her helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ioannis Angelakis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Angelakis, I., Austin, J.L. Aversive Events as Positive Reinforcers: An Investigation of Avoidance and Safety Signals in Humans. Psychol Rec 65, 627–635 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-015-0133-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-015-0133-4

Keywords

Navigation