Skip to main content
Log in

Exemplars and Categories Necessary for the Emergence of Intraverbals About Transitive Reasoning in Typically Developing Children

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research aimed to explore the effect of teaching Categories on the emergence of the intraverbals studied by Pérez-González, Herszlikowicz, and Williams (The Psychological Record 58;95–-129, 2008). Ten 6- and 7-year-old children were recruited and divided into 2 conditions. In Condition 1, 5 children learned intraverbals in which they had to say exemplars of 3 categories (e.g., “Name a continent”—”Europe”)—the Exemplars. They also learned intraverbals in which they had to say the categories of the exemplars named by the experimenter (e.g., “What is Europe”—”A continent”)—the Categories. Thereafter, they learned to say the countries corresponding to two cities (the A-B relations) and to say the continents of the two countries (the B-C relations). Finally, the intraverbals that result from combining A, B, and, C stimuli were probed, such as asking the cities corresponding to the continents. Five children in Condition 2 received the same experience, but they did not learn the Categories initially. Four of the 5 children of Condition 1 demonstrated the emergence of all probed ABC intraverbals, but no child of Condition 2 did so initially. These results indicate that learning Categories together with the Exemplars plays a strong role in the emergence of the probed ABC intraverbals. These findings reveals some basic learning processes involved in reasoning processes, such as transitive inference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alonso-Álvarez, B., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2006). Emergence of complex conditional discriminations by joint control of compound samples. The Psychological Record, 56, 447–463. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol56/iss3/8

  • Alonso-Álvarez, B., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2011). Derived control by compound and single stimuli in a matching-to-sample task in children. Psicothema, 23, 415–423. Retrieved from http://www.psicothema.com/pdf/3903.pdf

  • Alonso-Álvarez, B., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2013). Hierarchy among intersecting equivalence classes formed by unitary and compound stimuli. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 14, 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axe, J. B. (2008). Conditional discrimination in the intraverbal relation: A review and recommendations for future research. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 159–174.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belloso-Díaz, C., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2015a). Emergence of symmetrical intraverbals facilitated by learning skills with the intraverbal responses. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Belloso-Díaz, C., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2015b). Effect of learning tacts or tacts and intraverbals on the emergence of intraverbals about verbal categorization. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Braam, S. J., & Poling, A. (1983). Development of intraverbal behavior in mentally retarded individuals through transfer of stimulus control procedures: Classifications of verbal responses. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 4, 279–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carp, C. L., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2012). Effects of two training conditions of the emergence of novel intraverbals: An extension of Pérez-González et al. (2008). The Psychological Record, 62, 187–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2000). Matching compound samples with unitary comparisons: Derived stimulus relations in adults and children. The Psychological Record, 50, 671–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2002a). Establishing transfer of compound control in children: A stimulus control analysis. The Psychological Record, 52, 139–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2002b). Matching functionally same relations: Implications for equivalence-equivalence as a model for analogical reasoning. The Psychological Record, 52, 351–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, P. M., Johnson, K. R., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1985). Verbal relations within instruction: Are there subclasses of the intraverbal? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 301–313.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cihon, T. M. (2007). A review of training intraverbal repertories: Can precision teaching help? The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 123–133.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Du, L. (2015). Identification and establishment of reinforcers that make the development of complex social language possible. International Journal of Behavior Analysis and Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 13–34.

  • Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: Inducing and expanding new verbal capabilities in children with language delays. New York: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Yuan, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a multiple exemplar history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Hughes, J. C., & Lowe, C. F. (2006). Naming and categorization in young children: IV: Listener behavior training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 247–273. doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.125-04.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Harris, F. D. A. (2007). Naming and categorization in young children: V. Manual sign training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 367–381. doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.52-06.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-267.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple probe technique: A variation on the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189–196.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Junior, J. L., & Costa, G. G. (2003). Efeitos das respostas de observação diferenciais sobre a aprendizagem de relações condicionais com estímulos complexos [Effects of differential observing responses over the learning of conditional relations with complex stimuli]. Psichologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 16, 71–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Junior, J. L., Costa, G. G., Gonsales, L. F. S., & Golfeto, R. M. (2001). Aprendizagem e emergência de relações condicionais com estímulos modelos complexos [Learning and emergence of conditional relations with complex sample stimuli]. In H. J. Guilhardi, M. B. Pinho Madi, P. Piazzon Queiroz, & M. C. Scoz (Eds.), Sobre comportamento e cognição: Expondo a variabilidade (pp. 401–421). Sao Paulo, Brazil: ESETec Editores Asociados.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kisamore, A. N., Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2011). Training preschool children to use visual imagining as a problem-solving strategy for complex categorization tasks. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 255–278.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III. Vocal tact training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83, 47–65. doi:10.1901/jeab.2005.31-04.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1032

  • Mahoney, A. M., Miguel, C. F., Ahearn, W. H., & Bell, J. (2011). The role of common motor responses in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 237–262. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.95-237.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383–405.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Munnelly, A., Dymond, S., & Hinton, E. C. (2010). Relational reasoning with derived comparative relations: A novel model of transitive inference. Behavioural processes, 85, 8–17. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.05.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partington, J. W., & Bailey, J. S. (1993). Teaching of intraverbal behavior to preschool children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 11, 9–18.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-González, L. A. (1994). Transfer of relational stimulus control in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 487–503.

  • Pérez-González, L. A. (2015). Teaching verbal skills, facilitating emergence, and inducing capabilities for an effective intervention for people with autism. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Pérez-González, L. A., & Alonso-Álvarez, B. (2008). Common control by compound samples in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 81–101. doi:10.1901/jeab.2008.90-81.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-González, L. A., Belloso-Díaz, C., Caramés-Méndez, M., & Alonso-Álvarez, B. (2014a). Emergence of complex intraverbals determined by simpler intraverbals. The Psychological Record, 64, 509–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-González, L. A., Salameh, J., & García-Asenjo, L. (2014b). Unidirectional transfer between two types of intraverbals. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Pérez-González, L. A., García-Asenjo, L., Williams, G., & Carnerero, J. J. (2007). Emergence of intraverbal antonyms in children with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 697–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-González, L. A., Herszlikowicz, K., & Williams, G. (2008). Stimulus relations analysis and the emergence of novel intraverbals. The Psychological Record, 58, 95–129. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol58/iss1/7

  • Petursdottir, A. I., & Haflidadóttir, R. D. (2009). A Comparison of four strategies for teaching a small foreign-language vocabulary. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 685–690.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., Lechago, S. A., & Almason, S. M. (2008a). An evaluation of intraverbal training and listener training for teaching categorization skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 53–68.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petursdottir, A. I., Ólafsdóttir, A. R., & Aradóttir, B. (2008b). The effects of tact and listener training on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 411–415. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-411.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polson, D. A. D., & Parsons, J. A. (2000). Selection-based versus topography-based responding: An important distinction for stimulus equivalence? The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 17, 105–128.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sautter, R. A., LeBlanc, L. A., Jay, A. A., Goldsmith, T. R., & Carr, J. E. (2011). The role of problem solving in complex intraverbal repertoires. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 227–244.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M., Frank, M. J., Smith, A. C., Ly, S., & Carter, C. S. (2011). Transitive inference in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(3), 437–449. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0040-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, C. T., & Sundberg, M. L. (1990). Comparing topography-based verbal behavior with stimulus selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 31–41.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vignes, T. (2007). A comparison of topography-based and selection-based verbal behavior in typically developed children and developmentally disabled persons with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 113–122.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. L., Pack-Teixeira, L., & Howard, J. S. (1989). Teaching intraverbal behavior to severely retarded children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 7, 69–81.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werchan, D. M., & Gómez, R. L. (2013). Generalizing memories over time: Sleep and reinforcement facilitate transitive inference. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 100, 70–76. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grant PSI2009-08644, of the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación, Spain, to the second author. This research was conducted as partial fulfillment of the doctoral dissertation of the first author under the supervision of the second author. We thank Joanne Hill-Powell for editing and improving the reading of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luis Antonio Pérez-González.

Appendix A

Appendix A

Table 4 Correct responses out of 4 trials (or 2 trials when indicated by the asterisk) in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 1, in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), and after learning the AB, BC intraverbals, the Exemplars, and the Categories
Table 5 Correct responses out of 4 trials (or 2 trials when indicated by the asterisk) in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 2 conducted with Mary in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), after learning the AB, BC intraverbals and the Exemplars, and after learning the Categories
Table 6 Correct responses out of 4 trials in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 2 conducted with Lucy in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), after learning the AB, BC intraverbals and the Exemplars, and after learning the Categories
Table 7 Correct responses out of 4 trials in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 2 conducted with Lara in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), after learning the AB, BC intraverbals and the Exemplars, and after learning the Categories
Table 8 Correct responses out of 4 trials in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 2 conducted with Luis in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), after learning the AB, BC intraverbals and the Exemplars, and after learning the Categories
Table 9 Correct responses out of 4 trials in each intraverbal in each session of Condition 2 conducted with Jonny in the preintervention probes (“Pre”), after learning the AB, BC intraverbals and the Exemplars, and after learning the Categories

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Belloso-Díaz, C., Pérez-González, L.A. Exemplars and Categories Necessary for the Emergence of Intraverbals About Transitive Reasoning in Typically Developing Children. Psychol Rec 65, 541–556 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-015-0131-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-015-0131-6

Keywords

Navigation