Introduction

Children’s exposure to toxic metals such as lead (Pb), arsenic (As), mercury (Hg), cadmium (Cd), and manganese (Mn) is nearly ubiquitous [1, 2]. These prevalent exposures are a critical public health concern because even relatively low levels of metals, such as Pb, can disrupt normal development of the central nervous system, especially during fetal life and early childhood [36]. Environmental contributions to adverse neurodevelopmental and behavioral outcomes in children are a societal burden associated with substantial economic losses [79]. For example, the neurotoxic effects of childhood Pb exposure on decreased intelligence quotient (IQ) are estimated to cost up to $61 billion per year in economic losses in the USA [8]. While the neurodevelopmental effects of Pb and Hg exposure are well established, and increasing evidence is available for As [10, 11], other metals such as Cd and Mn are less well studied, and the economic burden associated with their health effects has not been estimated.

Importantly, metal exposure rarely occurs in isolation and co-exposure is likely the norm [12, 13]. Metal co-exposure also poses a critical threat to pediatric health [14]. The central nervous system is a common target organ for many environmental metals, leading to the natural question: What happens under joint exposure? Metals may interact to cause synergistic or antagonistic effects on neurodevelopment that are different from the main effects of exposure to each metal alone. Because the number of possible chemical combinations is infinite, researchers must make choices when addressing the role of mixed exposure and health. The epidemiologic literature is now beginning to address lower order interactions between metals, and new methodologies are being developed to address higher order mixtures [15, 16•]. Understanding the health effects of combinations of metals, as well as metal interactions with other chemical and non-chemical exposures, is critical for progressing the field of environmental health and protecting children’s health [17].

To summarize the state of the literature on the role of Cd, Mn, and metal mixtures as predictors of neurodevelopmental and behavioral outcomes in children, we conducted a review of epidemiologic studies examining early life exposure. We focus on Cd and Mn because they are pervasive metals with biological properties that could produce neurotoxic effects and they have not been reviewed recently. We also provide an updated summary of the rapidly growing mixture literature focused on neurodevelopmental outcomes and include a wider range of publication dates than previously reported [14]. We focus on early life, which we differentiate in this paper into two distinct windows—the prenatal period and childhood through adolescence. These life stages appear to be susceptibility windows for environmental metal exposures that place the child at increased risk for subsequent adverse health outcomes, due in part to life-stage-specific developmental processes. We present conclusions based on this body of evidence and provide recommendations for future studies.

Methods

We conducted a literature search to identify epidemiologic studies that examined the effects of Cd or Mn with respect to children’s neurodevelopmental/cognitive and behavioral outcomes using the following search terms: (child* OR infan* OR school OR postnatal OR post-natal OR prenatal OR pre-natal OR fetal OR pregnan* OR in utero) AND (neurodevelopment* OR cognit* OR mental OR intelligence OR behavior* OR behaviour* OR hyperactivity OR ADHD) AND (metal OR “manganese” [MeSH Terms] OR “cadmium” [MeSH Terms]). To identify studies on metal mixtures, we additionally used the following search terms: (interact* OR effect modif* OR mixture OR joint effect* OR additive effect*). We used PubMed and Web of Science search engines and restricted the search to peer-reviewed human studies published in English between January 2009 and March 2015. We further excluded studies that did not report a cognitive or behavioral outcome (for example, only reported motor effects) or were ecological or semi-ecological in design. Among the mixture literature, we excluded studies that did not report joint or interaction effects. We identified and summarized a total of 16, 17, and 16 studies on Cd, Mn, or metal mixtures, respectively. For the Cd- and Mn-specific literature, all reported biomarker concentrations were converted to microgram per liter or microgram per gram to allow comparisons across studies. It should be noted that some studies measured other metals such as Pb, Hg, and As, but the findings with cognitive or behavioral outcomes are not reported herein unless an interaction effect was presented. Biomarker concentrations were omitted from the mixture literature table for brevity; we direct the reader to the original article for these details.

Sources of Exposure

In order to fully understand the public health impact of toxic metals, an understanding of their exposure sources is needed. Tobacco is the dominant source of Cd exposure among smokers; however, the primary source of exposure in the general non-smoking population is diet (seafood, organ meats, grains, leafy vegetables, and root crops) [18, 19]. Anthropogenic sources of Cd also include industrial smelters, phosphate fertilizers, and nickel-cadmium batteries [19]. Although the placenta protects the fetus by restricting Cd, Cd levels can be detectable in newborn cord blood despite the limited transfer at the maternal-fetal interface [2022]. Postnatal Cd exposure can also occur through secondhand tobacco smoke, infant formula [23, 24], or breast milk [25], and the primary source of Cd in childhood is dietary.

Diet is the primary source of Mn exposure in the general population (grains, nuts, tea, etc.) [2628], although only ∼3–5 % of ingested Mn is typically absorbed and elimination is tightly regulated through homeostatic mechanisms [29, 30]. Excess exposure most commonly occurs from elevated Mn levels in drinking water or in air sometimes as the result of naturally occurring processes but more commonly due to industrial activities such as welding, mining, steel manufacturing, and combustion of Mn-containing gasoline additives [28]. Mn, like a host of metals including Cd, can be bound to particulate matter, such as particulate matter less than 2.5 μm in diameter (PM2.5) found in ambient pollution. Mn is also a component of two extensively applied agricultural fungicides—maneb and mancozeb [31, 32]. Because Mn is an essential element, it is actively transported across the placenta from mother to fetus [33], which results in higher levels in cord blood than in maternal blood [34, 35]. Postnatal Mn exposure can occur through breast milk [36] and infant formula [37]. Because Mn is processed and excreted in the liver, oral exposures are per dose less toxic than ambient air exposure. Inhaled Mn aerosols from showering may contribute to exposure [38, 39], though primary sources of exposure during childhood and adolescence are diet and drinking water.

Epidemiologic Evidence for Neurotoxic Effects of Cadmium

Cognitive Effects

We identified ten studies that examined early life Cd exposure and neurodevelopmental or cognitive outcomes (Supplement 1) [4049]. Of these studies, five were prospective, four were cross sectional, and one was a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial design. Exposure to Cd was measured at multiple life stages: Prenatal exposure was examined in seven studies [40, 4347, 49], early childhood exposure (1 to 3 years of age) was measured in one study [41], and two studies evaluated exposure in school-age children (6 to 15 year olds) [42, 48]. A single longitudinal study measured exposure prenatally and at 5 years [47]. Cd levels were measured in blood (n = 6), urine (n = 4), hair (n = 1), and placenta (n = 1). Among these, prenatal Cd exposure was estimated with cord blood (n = 3), maternal blood during pregnancy (n = 2), maternal urine (n = 2), and placenta (n = 1). A single study measured both urine and hair and reported significant associations with urine, but not hair Cd levels [48]. Another study measured prenatal Cd in three matrices— maternal blood at delivery, placenta, and cord blood—and reported significant associations only with cord blood levels [40]. Five studies examined the relationship between Cd biomarkers and IQ [41, 45, 4749], and five studies examined other neurodevelopmental outcome measures.

Four of five studies identified a significant inverse relationship between prenatal/childhood Cd and IQ [45, 4749]. A prospective study of 1305 mother-child pairs identified significant inverse associations between maternal first trimester urine Cd and child full scale, verbal, and performance IQ at 5 years; associations with concurrent child urine Cd were weaker but still significant [47]. In a prospective cohort of 106 children, a significant inverse association with performance and full-scale IQ at 4.5 years was observed among children with high (>median) cord blood Cd [49]. A prospective cohort of 119 mother-child pairs found a significant inverse association between maternal blood Cd measured in early pregnancy and performance IQ at 5 years [45]. In a cross-sectional study of 261 6 to 9 year olds, urine Cd but not hair Cd was inversely associated with verbal comprehension on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)-IV (boys and girls) and with full-scale IQ in boys only [48]. Lastly, a longitudinal study of 441 children identified suggestive trends of inverse relationships between 2-year blood Cd levels and IQ at 5 and 7 years, although these associations were not statistically significant [41].

Five studies examined associations between Cd exposure and other neurodevelopmental outcome measures. Prenatal [46] and early life [41] Cd exposure was examined in relation to scores on the Mental Development Index (MDI) of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID), but no significant relationship was found. A prospective cohort study of 4 year olds in Spain found a suggestive but non-significant association between prenatal Cd and the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) measure of cognition [43]. Two studies identified a significant inverse relationship between cord blood Cd and Apgar score at 1 and/or 5 min [40, 44]. Lastly, a large cross-sectional study in the USA noted significant associations between urinary Cd levels and learning disorders/disability and special education among 6 to 15 year olds [42]. Taken together, these recent studies on IQ and other neurodevelopmental outcomes are suggestive of a potential detrimental effect of early life Cd on cognitive ability that may also manifest as impaired learning though the epidemiologic evidence is limited.

Behavioral Effects

We identified nine studies that assessed early life Cd exposure and behavioral outcomes [4143, 48, 5054]: Four were cross sectional, two were prospective, two were case control, and one was a randomized clinical trial design. Cd was measured in blood (n = 5), urine (n = 3), or hair (n = 2), predominantly during early to late childhood (5 to 18 year olds). Two studies assessed prenatal Cd levels in maternal urine or cord blood [43, 52], and one measured early childhood exposure (2 years of age) [41]. Five studies examined associations between Cd and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or ADHD-like behavior [42, 43, 51, 53, 54], and four assessed other behavioral scales [41, 48, 50, 52].

It is notable that none of the five studies that examined the association between early life Cd and ADHD reported significant associations [42, 43, 51, 53, 54]. Two of the studies were cross sectional, two used a case-control design, and one was a prospective cohort. ADHD was assessed using multiple formats, either by parental report or by administering established ADHD rating scale tests. While three studies had limited sample sizes with less than 100 cases, two were large population-based studies with relatively large sample sizes (>300) that measured prenatal or concurrent urinary levels of Cd [42, 43]. In the absence of a meta-analysis, the consistent lack of association in the current literature between early life Cd and childhood ADHD is suggestive, although not conclusive, evidence of a null association.

Two of four studies reported significant findings between prenatal or concurrent Cd exposure and other scales of child behavior [50, 52]. A prospective study of Belgian children noted a significant increased risk of emotional problems in 7- to 8-year-old boys following prenatal exposure [52]. In a large cross-sectional study in China, hair Cd levels were associated with withdrawn, social, and attention problems among 7 to 16 year olds [50]. However, in a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial in the USA, no significant associations were observed between blood Cd levels at 2 years and a battery of behavior and attention tests conducted subsequently at 5 and 7 years [41]. In another cross-sectional study, no significant associations were found between urine or hair Cd and a battery of behavior and attention tests at 6 to 9 years of age [48]. Overall, while prenatal or school-age Cd exposure may impact childhood behavior, data are extremely limited and further research is needed.

Epidemiologic Evidence for Neurotoxic Effects of Manganese

Cognitive Effects

We identified 14 studies that examined the association between Mn and neurodevelopmental or cognitive outcomes in neonates or children (Supplement 2) [5567, 68••]. Most studies were cross sectional (n = 10), and four were prospective cohorts. Mn was measured in blood (n = 10), hair (n = 7), or drinking water (n = 1). Both hair and blood biomarkers were used in three studies, and all of these reported significant associations with neurodevelopmental outcomes for hair but not blood Mn [63, 65, 66].

All four of the prospective studies reviewed support the notion that the prenatal and early postnatal period may be a sensitive developmental window for Mn exposure with regard to neurodevelopment [58, 61, 67, 68••]. In a large prospective cohort study of neonates from China, a cord serum Mn threshold of 5 μg/L was identified, above which 3 day olds showed cognitive deficits as measured by the Neonatal Behavioral Neurological Assessment (NBNA) [67]. However, there are well-known limitations to assessing neonates, given the limited neurologic repertoire inherent in newborns. Two studies reported an inverted U-shaped association between blood Mn levels measured at delivery or at 12 months of age and mental development scores on the BSID at ages 6 to 12 months [58, 68••], indicating the potential for Mn to act as both an essential element and a toxic metal. Chung et al. reported a transition from beneficial to adverse effects on 6-month mental development scores when maternal blood Mn levels were 24–28 μg/L [68••]. Finally, a prospective cohort study of 2-year-old children in Taiwan reported that cord blood Mn was associated with decrements in cognitive and language subscales of the Comprehensive Developmental Inventory for Infants and Toddlers (CDIIT) [61]. Although additional study is needed, these findings collectively suggest that prenatal and early postnatal Mn exposure might be a particular concern for early childhood development. If a toxic threshold during pregnancy is confirmed, guidance levels of environmental or dietary Mn exposure during pregnancy should be studied.

The majority (12 of 14) of studies examining childhood Mn exposure observed significant inverse associations with IQ or other mental development indices (BSID, CDIIT, etc.) [5661, 6367, 68••]. These studies were conducted in various geographic locations and populations, had moderate sample sizes (many with n > 200), and adjusted for important covariates (e.g., maternal education, child age, child gender, and nutritional status). Two cross-sectional studies in adolescents that measured blood Mn reported no significant association with IQ [55, 62], although one of these reported significant associations with deficits on the Learning Disability Evaluation scale (LDES) [55]. Another cross-sectional study found that hair Mn, but not blood Mn, was associated with long-term memory and learning on the Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (CAVLT) among 7 to 11 year olds [66]. Despite a limited sample size (n = 60), a cross-sectional study of 1- to 4-year-old Uruguayan children reported significant inverse associations of hair Mn with cognition and language, especially among girls, but only in unadjusted models [64]. Taken together, these studies support the conclusion that elevated early life Mn exposure adversely impacts childhood cognition with particularly consistent associations with IQ.

Behavioral Effects

We identified seven studies that examined the association between early life Mn and child/adolescent behavior [54, 55, 57, 62, 6971]. The studies used cross-sectional (n = 4), case-control (n = 2), and case-cohort (n = 1) designs. One study examined prenatal exposure and six studies assessed exposure in early to late childhood. Five studies assessed Mn exposure using blood, one used hair [57], one measured drinking water Mn levels [71], and one measured Mn in teeth [69]. Three of the studies examined the relationship with ADHD, one examined associations with ASD, and three assessed other behavior or attention scales.

Among the three studies of ADHD, a case-control study in the United Arab Emirates reported increased odds of ADHD with increased blood Mn levels [54]. However, this study had a small sample size (n = 92, with only 18 cases of ADHD) and did not adjust for any confounding variables. In a large cross-sectional study of South Korean children, blood Mn levels were associated with poorer scores of commission on one of three ADHD tests, but there was no association with doctor-diagnosed ADHD [55]. A case-cohort study in Brazil found that the treatment of adolescent ADHD with the common medication methylphenidate (Ritalin®) significantly reduced blood Mn levels [70]. While it remains unclear whether ADHD medication use potentially affects the concomitant levels of other toxic metals, this is an intriguing finding. A recent study found that methylphenidate administered following chronic postnatal Mn exposure resulted in improved motor function in rats; however, there was no effect on Mn blood levels [72]. If Mn metabolism is part of the underlying biologic pathway for ADHD, this finding may be evidence for a biological role of Mn in ADHD causation. Conversely, if methylphenidate alters Mn metabolism independent of its effect on ADHD, this may represent a source of bias. Further research is needed to replicate and understand this relationship.

A case-control study of children in the USA with ASD found that cases had marginally significantly lower levels of enamel Mn, representing postnatal exposure, compared to controls [69]. These findings should be interpreted cautiously, however, because tooth enamel does not track as well as tooth dentine with early life timing of exposure due to its longer maturation process [73]. Nonetheless, the role of teeth as biomarkers for metal exposure deserves further study. Three studies examining scales of adolescent behavior other than ASD or ADHD report conflicting results [57, 62, 71]. A cross-sectional study of 11- to 14-year-old Italian adolescents found that blood Mn was not associated with any of the child behavior checklist (CBCL) subscales [62], whereas a cross-sectional study of 8 to 11 year olds in Bangladesh reported a significant association between drinking water Mn, but not blood Mn, with internalizing and externalizing behaviors [71]. A cross-sectional study of 7 to 12 year olds in Brazil found significantly impaired performance on attention when comparing higher hair Mn tertiles to lower Mn but did not report a significant linear relationship [57].

Taken together, these studies provide some evidence of a link between early life Mn and ADHD, ASD, or other behavioral outcomes in children but are not conclusive. Future studies should explore the strengths and limitations of various exposure biomarkers, including the use of teeth as a biomarker to retrospectively address exposure timing and more clearly distinguish prenatal versus postnatal exposures. Another knowledge gap is whether medication use alters Mn blood levels directly or as a consequence of therapeutic effects.

Epidemiologic Evidence for Neurotoxic Effects of Metal Mixtures

Cognitive Effects

We identified 13 articles that examined metal mixture effects on cognition and/or neurodevelopment (Supplement 3) [16•, 42, 46, 6062, 74, 75, 76•, 7780]. Among these studies, six were cross sectional, six were prospective, and one was retrospective. Exposure to metal mixtures was measured at various life stages: Prenatal exposure was examined in six studies [16•, 46, 61, 77, 79, 80], early childhood exposure (1 to 3 years of age) was measured in two studies [74, 76•], and the remaining five studies evaluated exposure in school-age children (6 to 15 year olds) [42, 60, 62, 75, 78]. Metal exposure was estimated primarily with biomarkers (blood, hair, and urine), though three studies measured metals in environmental samples (drinking water, air, and soil).

Most studies (n = 11) estimated effects of binary combinations of metals on neurodevelopment, even when exposure to more than two metals was measured (e.g., [61, 77]). Most likely, this limitation is a function of sample size and the limited statistical approaches available to examine higher order interactions. Pb is the most commonly examined metal when assessing interactions with other metals as a mixture: Eight studies considered joint effects of Pb with As, Cd, Mn, or Hg [42, 46, 6062, 74, 77, 80]. Lead-Mn interactions were estimated in four studies, three of which reported significant adverse interactive effects. A prospective study of 230 Taiwanese children reported an interaction between prenatal Pb and Mn exposure measured in cord blood on scores of the CDIIT at 2 years of age [61]. In this study, in utero exposure to high Pb and high Mn was associated with larger deficits in cognition and language development compared to low exposure to both metals or to exposure to high levels of only one metal at a time. In the early postnatal period, a significant Pb-Mn interaction was similarly observed in a cohort of Mexican children [74], where slopes for the effect of 1-year blood Pb on both mental and psychomotor development scores of the BSID-II were steeper among children with high blood Mn levels (highest quintile Mn vs. middle levels). Among 8- to 11-year old Korean children, Pb exposure was associated with lower concurrently measured IQ scores but only among children with high blood Mn levels [60]. In contrast to the aforementioned three studies, no statistical interaction was found between Pb and Mn on IQ in 11- to 14-year-old Italian children, despite considering multiple measures of Mn exposure (blood, hair, air, and soil) [62]. We should note that because this study was cross sectional, it did not have information on early childhood Pb exposure (average blood Pb was 1.7 μg/dL at 11 to 14 years of age). Taken together, the evidence to date suggests that Mn may exacerbate the well-documented neurotoxic effects of Pb among children, particularly at younger ages. Possible mechanisms for this interaction include increased binding affinity for Pb in the brain in the presence of excess Mn levels [81] or joint interference with the release of dopamine or other neurotransmitters [82, 83].

Interactions between Pb and Cd have been examined in two studies [42, 46]. In a large cohort of 884 mother-infant pairs in Korea, maternal blood Pb and blood Cd levels measured during pregnancy were related to scores on the BSID-II among 6-month-old infants [46]. Increasing Pb exposure was associated with lower mental and psychomotor development scores among infants with high (>median) prenatal Cd exposure, in particular for exposures during late pregnancy. In contrast, no interaction between blood Pb and urinary Cd levels was observed in relation to learning disability or special education utilization among 6- to 15-year-old participants of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) [42].

Additional evidence that Pb neurotoxicity may be modified in the presence of other metals comes from a prospective study of Pb and methylmercury [80] and a retrospective study of Pb and As [77]. An antagonistic interaction between prenatal Pb and Hg exposure, measured in cord blood, was reported on verbal learning, memory, and attention evaluated in children from the Faroe Islands at 7 and/or 14 years of age, suggesting that high methylmercury exposure may mask adverse Pb effects. An explanation offered by the authors is that these metals may have parallel mechanisms of action that work competitively [80]. Co-exposure to Pb and As in soil at maternal residences during pregnancy was estimated using geospatial statistical models then regressed in relation to intellectual disability (ID) in ∼8 to 12 year olds [77]. Joint exposure to high Pb and As in the prenatal period was associated with a higher probability of ID among children with normal birth weight for gestational age compared to low joint exposure or high exposure to one metal at a time.

Two-way interactions between Mn and either selenium or As have been explored in at least three studies [75, 78, 79]. Selenium, an essential nutrient like Mn, is an antioxidant and was found to be protective against adverse effects of excess prenatal Mn exposure, measured in cord serum, on scores of the NBNA [79]. Arsenic, which may be neurotoxic on its own, did not interact with Mn on intellectual function or academic achievement in two cross-sectional studies in Bangladesh, where 8- to 11-year-old children were exposed to elevated levels of naturally occurring As and Mn in drinking water [75, 78]. If true, the lack of a Mn-As interaction may indicate that these metals have distinct biologic mechanisms of toxicity. It is also possible that the main effects of these metals at higher exposure levels, such as those observed in these Bangladeshi cohorts, overwhelm any interactive effect. Further, at lower exposure levels, a Mn-As interaction has been previously reported on IQ, verbal learning, and memory among 11- to 13-year-old US children [84].

The two studies that estimated neurodevelopmental effects of more than two metals at a time [16•, 76•] were heterogeneous in their populations, study design, biomarkers used to estimate exposure, neurodevelopmental outcomes examined, and statistical methods applied to evaluate combined effects. With prospective data from an ongoing study of Bangladeshi children, Bayesian kernel machine regression was used to flexibly model the exposure-response function of an arsenic-lead-manganese mixture [16•]. The authors reported an inverted U-shaped association between cord blood Mn and the 2-year motor composite score from the BSID-III at the median cord blood Pb level, though this effect was only observed in the middle levels of As exposure. No biological explanation was provided for this finding (as this was a biostatistic method paper), but an inverted U relationship that reflects Mn essentiality at low levels and toxicity at high levels could be disrupted by higher levels of a co-occurring toxicant such as As. Cross-sectional associations of As, Pb, Mn, and Cd with scores on the BSID-III were examined among preschool children in Uruguay [76•]. Children were assigned to four latent clusters (low metal exposure, low-to-moderate metal exposure, high lead and cadmium, and high metal exposure) according to their metal exposure characteristics, and these clusters were modeled in relation to cognitive and language scores. No associations were observed, although the sample size was small (n = 92 with imputed data) and the variability around estimates was large.

Behavioral Effects

We identified five articles that reported metal mixture effects on behavioral outcomes [42, 62, 71, 85, 86•]. One study used a prospective design to evaluate prenatal and childhood metal mixture exposures in relation to impulsivity [85]; three studies used a cross-sectional design to investigate ADHD, ADHD-like behaviors, and other externalizing and internalizing behaviors [42, 62, 75]; and one case-control study examined ASD [86•]. Metal exposure was estimated with biomarkers (blood, hair, and urine) as well as environmental samples (drinking water, air, soil).

Four studies evaluated binary combinations of metals [42, 62, 71, 85]. Again, Pb was the most commonly examined metal with respect to interactions with one other metal (Hg, Cd, and Mn). Prenatal Pb exposure, measured in umbilical cord blood, was associated with increased impulsivity among 9 to 13 year olds that was enhanced in the presence of higher Hg exposure [85]. This synergistic Pb-Hg interaction on response inhibition is in contrast to the antagonistic Pb-Hg interaction observed for cognition among children 7 and 14 years of age from the Faroe Islands [80]. No interactions between Pb and Mn were observed on self-reported behavior on the Conners-Wells’ Scale among Italian adolescents [62], and interactions between Pb and Cd on ADHD among 6 to 15 year olds were not statistically significant [42]. In a cross-sectional study of 8-to 11 year olds from Bangladesh, no statistical interactions were observed between Mn and As exposure on behavior [71].

Only one study estimated joint effects of more than two metals on behavior [86•]. Roberts et al. assessed perinatal exposure to ambient concentrations of antimony, As, Cd, chromium, Pb, Mn, Hg, and nickel, as well as other non-metal pollutants, based on US EPA dispersion models [86•]. Associations between ASD and overall metal exposure were estimated in two ways: An overall measure of metal concentrations was derived by summing the quintile category score for each metal, and an overall estimate of association was derived by pooling odds ratios (ORs) from individual metals. The overall metal score and the pooled OR for metals were both positively associated with ASD, suggesting that perinatal exposure to multiple metals may increase the risk of ASD.

Recommendations for Future Directions

There is a growing body of literature supporting the neurodevelopmental toxicity of Mn exposure. For Cd, the literature is sparser, but there is some evidence that Cd may be neurotoxic to children and more research is needed. The metal mixture literature is beginning to identify interactive and joint effects on cognition; however, there is a general lack of studies on behavioral effects. There remain many research gaps, including selecting proper exposure biomarkers, addressing the importance of exposure timing and the role of life stage on neurobehavioral testing, collecting prospective data to evaluate persistence of possible effects, and implementing novel statistical modeling to disentangle mixture effects. It may be unrealistic for the research community to systematically address all of these issues, but the role of each should be taken into consideration when assessing new studies on these metals.

Selection of Biomarkers

No single biomarker clearly stands out when studying Cd or Mn toxicity. In the studies reviewed, Cd was measured most often in blood or urine and less frequently in hair or placenta. Once steady state is reached, Cd in blood has a half-life of over 10 years [18] and represents recent as well as past exposure. Urine Cd reflects cumulative exposure as well as the kidney-specific burden [87]. In the Mn studies reviewed, Mn was measured in blood, hair, drinking water, and tooth enamel. Blood Mn generally reflects recent and acute exposure, whereas hair reflects past exposure and integrates exposure over several months. Because Mn is an essential nutrient, blood Mn is under tight regulatory control. Blood Mn may then only reflect body burden in relatively extreme cases of overload or deficiency. Evidence from occupational studies shows that high workplace Mn exposure correlates with blood Mn among subjects [88], although this has not been consistent and some data suggest that the utility of blood Mn as an exposure biomarker may depend on the exposure parameters [89]. Nevertheless, many studies have found relationships between blood Mn and neurological outcomes. Although not represented in the literature herein, toenail Mn also represents a biomarker of cumulative exposure that integrates exposure from 7 months to 1 year in the past [90]. Mn is also found in teeth, which possess daily growth rings analogous to those found in trees and allow for more precise determination of exposure timing. In contrast to tooth enamel, dentine Mn has recently been validated, reflects a child’s longitudinal metal exposure prenatally through 6–12 years of life [91], and correlates with environmental measures [92] and with cord blood [73]. Therefore, dentine (tooth) Mn holds promise as a sensitive and precise biomarker of exposure in early life [91].

Timing of Exposure and Sensitive Developmental Windows

Neurodevelopmental processes such as neurogenesis, anatomic organization of neurons/synapses, synaptogenesis, and synaptic pruning are at their peak during fetal life and childhood relative to adult life [93]. Most of these processes also occur through adolescence as well. Central nervous system development underlies the sensitivity of children to toxic chemicals [94]. It is therefore critical to consider timing of environmental exposures because certain developmental periods may be more sensitive to the effects of environmental exposures than others. For example, animal studies have shown that prenatal Mn exposure had greater effects on markers of inflammatory neurotoxicity (reduced glutathione and metallothionein gene expression) among neonatal rats compared to older rats [95, 96].

These issues are further complicated if one considers mixture effects, as mixed effects are just as likely as main effects to be dependent on exposure timing and each chemical may act differentially depending on the age of exposure. In this regard, laser ablation-inductively coupled plasma-mass spectrometry analysis of deciduous teeth holds particular promise [97]. New methods that can objectively reconstruct past exposure to multiple metals and their mixtures will allow researchers to overcome many of the research gaps noted above. More research is needed with prospective exposure data that can assess the role of windows of susceptibility, especially for behavioral outcomes. Specific knowledge gaps in the mixture literature include a lack of data among young (1–3 years old) children and adolescents (∼15–19 year olds).

Development not only induces sensitivity to chemicals but also plays a role in interpreting exposure biomarkers. For example, blood Mn is highest in infancy and remains higher than the average adult level until approximately 12 years of age and then gradually declines through adulthood [98]. These age-specific shifts in blood Mn levels are significantly more pronounced among females than males [98]. Future research estimating body burden of Mn should consider these age- and gender-specific differences in exposure biomarkers and the role of Mn as an essential nutrient during development.

The Role of Life Stage on Neurobehavioral Testing

Unlike adults, nearly all behavioral assessment tools in children are age specific. This is because not all functions are present at birth but are expressed at different life stages and deficits from early life exposure may not be measurable for years. IQ cannot be assessed in a toddler, for example. Neonatal phenotypes have a large amount of measurement error, simply because neonates have limited variability in their behaviors. Associations with toxic exposures among neonates are generally less stable over time than relationships seen at older ages. This property, while less pronounced, holds when assessing cognition and behavior in children and adolescents. Moreover, many traits are not expressed until a specific life stage. For example, internalizing behaviors are more easily measured in a 9 year old than in a toddler but are most prevalent in adolescence (i.e., traits themselves are life stage specific). If we are to understand the role of exposure timing, future work should develop better neurobehavioral assessment tools at younger ages to allow for repeated measurements of functional domains at multiple life stages.

Complex Statistical Modeling

Although an increasing number of epidemiologic studies are considering metal co-exposure as a potential confounder, fewer studies have evaluated joint, interactive, or modifying effects of higher order co-exposure (i.e., three or more chemicals). This may be due, at least in part, to challenges related to choosing a biomarker or measure of exposure that is appropriate for all metals of interest, difficulties related to collecting multiple biomarkers, the cost of analyzing multiple metals and/or multiple exposure measures, and statistical challenges such as limited power and analysis of highly correlated data [14]. In the present mixture literature, there is little consistency in terms of metals evaluated, measures of exposure used for each metal, and statistical approach employed for analyzing joint or interactive effects. Methodologies to address higher order mixtures such as weighted quantile sum regression [15] and Bayesian kernel machine regression [16•] will help to address some knowledge gaps.

Conclusion

This review of the current literature suggests that early life Cd and Mn exposure leads to reductions in childhood cognitive ability. There does not appear to be evidence of a relationship between Cd and ADHD, but a small number of studies provide suggestive evidence of a relationship with other negative behavioral outcomes. There is suggestive evidence of a link between Mn with ADHD, autism, and other behavioral outcomes. Limited epidemiologic evidence suggests that joint exposure to multiple metals may have different, and in many cases worse, effects on neurodevelopment and cognition than effects of individual metals, in particular when the mixture includes Pb.