Skip to main content
Log in

Do They Speak Language?

  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The question: are humans the only animals endowed with language? must be preceded by the question: what makes language a unique communication system? The American linguist Charles F. Hockett answers the second question by listing what he considers the criteria that differentiate language from other communication systems. His ‘design-feature’ approach, first presented in 1958, has become a popular tool by which the communication systems of non-human animals are guaranteed a priori exclusion from the notion of language. However, the results of interspecific communication research and the discovery of language–like qualities in the natural communication systems of non-human animals (as opposed to the artificial systems devised to further language research) demonstrate that language capabilities have evolved in parallel in many species. Thus Hockett’s approach is thoroughly undermined, and in need of revision. The more fundamental question that must be faced by the design-feature approach is: are its features essential for language as a distinct and vivid phenomenon, or merely applied to language as an object of linguistic investigation? This paper offers a detailed overview of Hockett’s design-features and emphasizes the problematic nature of certain characteristics. Following Slobodchikoff and Segerdahl et al., the paper shows that language cannot be defined as an exclusive quality of a single species.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. However, the very first public mention of those seven design-features emerged the previous year, in his ’A Course in Modern Linguistic.’

  2. “The meaningful elements in any language – ‘words’ in everyday parlance, ‘morphemes’ to the linguist – constitute an enormous stock.” (Hockett 1982:6) Nevertheless, words and morphemes are not synonymous. For example, the word animals has two morphemes animal (base morpheme = morpheme giving a meaning to the word) and s (suffix = a morpheme that comes after a base morpheme).

  3. DF 1 = Design-Feature 1; and so forth.

  4. Abstract symbols representing words

  5. LRC—Language Research Center – (Segerdahl et al. 2005).

References

  • Abe, K., & Watanabe, D. (2011). Songbirds possess the spontaneous ability to discriminate syntactic rules. Nature Neuroscience. doi:10.1038/nn.2869.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, C., & Bekoff, M. (1997). Species of mind: The philosophy and biology of cognitive ethology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, S. A. (1967). The structure of primate social communication. In S. A. Altmann (Ed.), Social communication among primates (pp. 325–362). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2013). We are animals and therein lies hope for a better future. Blog post. Psychology Today. http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/animal–emotions/201312/we–are–animals–and–therein–lies–hope–better–future. Accessed 25 Jul 2014.

  • Bickerton, D. (2000). Resolving discontinuity: a minimalist distinction between human and non-human minds. Animal Zoologist, 40(6), 862–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchler, J. (Ed.). (1955). Philosophical writings of Peirce. New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burling, R. (1993). Primate calls, human language, and nonverbal communication. Current Anthropology, (34)1, 25–53.

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago: The University Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crist, E. (2004). Can an insect speak? The case of the honeybee dance language. Social Studies of Science, (34)1, 7–43.

  • Deacon, W. T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1991). “Eating well, “or the calculation of the subject: An Interview with Jacques Derrida. In E. Cadava, P. Connor, & J. L. Nancy (Eds.), Who comes after the subject? (pp. 96–111). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fouts, R. (2004). Apes, Darwinian continuity and the law. Animal Law, 10, 99–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E. C., Gedamke, J., Rekdahl, M. L., Noad, M. J., Garrigue, C., & Gales, N. (2013). Humpback whale song on the southern ocean feeding grounds: implications for cultural transmission. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature04675.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J. L. (1976). The dance-language controversy. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 51(2), 211–244.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. R. [1976] (1981). The question of animal awareness: Evolutionary continuity of mental experience. New York: The Rockefeller University Press.

  • Hailman, J. P., & Ficken, M. S. (1986). Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘language’ by structural linguistics. Animal Behaviour, 34(6), 1899–1901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1988). How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: the role of experience. Behaviour, 105(3/4), 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. F. (1959). Animal ‘languages’ and human language. In J. N. Spuhler (Ed.), The evolution of man’s capacity for culture (pp. 32–39). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. F. (1963). The problems of universals in language. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals in language (pp. 1–29). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. F. (1977) [1960b]. Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In C. F Hockett, The view from language: Selected essays 1948–1974 (pp. 124–162). Athens, GA: University of Georgia Press.

  • Hockett, C. F. (1982) [1960a]. The origin of speech. In W. S-Y.Wang (Ed.), Human communication: Language and its psychobiological bases (pp. 4–12). San Francisco: Freeman.

  • Hurford, J. R. (2004). Human uniqueness, learned symbols and recursive thought. European Review, 12(4), 551–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleisner, K. (2008). The semantic morphology of Adolf Portmann: a starting point for the biosemiotics of organic form? Biosemiotics, 1(2), 207–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleisner, K. (2011). Perceive, co-opt, modify, and live! Organism as a centre of experience. Biosemiotics, 4(2), 223–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleisner, K., & Maran, T. (2014). Visual communication in animals: Applying Portmannian and Uexküllian biosemiotic approach. In D. Machin (Ed.), Visual communication (pp. 659–676). Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroodsma, D. (2004). The diversity and plasticity of birdsong. In P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn (Eds.), Nature’s music: The science of birdsong (pp. 108–131). San Diego: Elsevier academic press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kull, K. (2010). Ecosystems are made of semiosic bonds: consortia, umwelten, biophony and ecological codes. Biosemiotics, 3(3), 347–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenthal, F., & Lefebvre, L. (Eds.). (2014). Language and recursion. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangum, W. A. (2010). The “Language”of honey bees. In S. J. Behrens & J. A. Parker (Eds.), Language in the real world: An introduction to linguistic (pp. 255–273). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manser, M. B., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2002). Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 55–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maran, T., & Kleisner, K. (2010). Towards an evolutionary biosemiotics: semiotic selection and semiotic co-option. Biosemiotics, 3(2), 189–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinelli, D. (2010). A critical companion to zoosemiotics: people, paths, ideas. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R. I. (2002). Cognitive development in deaf children: The interface of language and perception in neuropsychology. In S. J. Segalowitz & I. Rapin (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 8, Part II., pp. 71–107). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nöth, W. (1990). Handbook of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nöth, W. (2013). The life of symbols and other legisigns: More than a mere metaphor? In V. Romanini & E. Fernandáz (Eds.), Peirce and Biosemiotics: A guess at the riddle of life (pp. 171–183). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Outtara, K., Lemasson, A., & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007808.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, F., & Linden, E. (1981). The education of Koko. New York: Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, Ch. S. (1998a) [1902]. The ethics of terminology. In Peirce edition project (Ed.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings (Vol. 2, 1893–1913, pp. 263–267). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

  • Peirce, Ch. S. (1998b) [1903]. Nomenclature and divisions of triadic relations, as far as they are determined. In Peirce edition project (Ed.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings (Vol. 2, 1893–1913, pp. 289–299). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition, 95, 201–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. (2004). Is language the key to human intelligence? Science. doi:10.1126/science.1093993.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–382.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthe, S. N. (2014). Creating the umwelt: from chance to choice. Biosemiotics. doi:10.1007/s12304-014-9204-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Padden, C. (2011). The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 29(2), 503–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2004). The gentle genius of bonobos. Online video clip. TED. <http://www.ted.com/talks/susan_savage_rumbaugh_on_apes_that_write.html>. Accessed on 25 Nov 2013.

  • Sebeok, T. A. (1972). Perspectives in zoosemiotics. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebeok, T. A. (Ed.). (1977). How animals communicate. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebeok, T. A. (1990). Essays in zoosemiotics. Toronto: Toronto Semiotic Circle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segerdahl, P., Fields, W., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2005). Kanzi’s primal language: The cultural initiation of primates into language. Hampshire: Palgrave MacMillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1993). Meaning, reference and intentionality in the natural vocalizations of monkeys. In H. L. Roitblat, L. M. Herman, & P. E. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communication: Comparative perspectives. Hillsday: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210(4471), 801–803.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff, C. (2012). Chasing Doctor Dolittle: Learning the language of animals. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff, C., Perla, B. S., & Verdolin, J. (2009). Prairie Dogs: Communication and community in an animal society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stella, M., & Kleisner, K. (2010). Uexküllian umwelt as science and as ideology: the light and the dark side of a concept. Theory in Biosciences, 129(1), 39–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, C., & Zuberbühler, K. (2014). Predation affects alarm call usage in female Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana diana). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68(2), 321–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. New York: University of Buffalo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, T. N. (2014). Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Animal Behaviour. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallerman, M., & Gibson, K. R. (Eds.). (2012). The Oxford handbook of language evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thibault, P. J. (2011). First-order languaging dynamics and second-order language: the distributed language view. Ecological Psychology, 23(3), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1972). The Comparison of vocal communication in animals and man. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal communication (pp. 27–47). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tønnessen, M. (2009). Umwelt transitions: uexküll and environmental change. Biosemiotics, 2(1), 47–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Hulst, H. (Ed.). (2010). Recursion and human language. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (1957) [1934]. A Stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior (pp. 5–80). New York: International Universities Press.

  • Webb, D. M., & Zhang, J. (2005). FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals. Journal of Heredity, 96(3), 212–216.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Survivor signals: The biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. In M. Naguib, V. Janik, N. Clayton, & K. Zuberbühler (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (pp. 277–322). London: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lucie Čadková.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Čadková, L. Do They Speak Language?. Biosemiotics 8, 9–27 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-014-9225-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-014-9225-9

Keywords

Navigation