Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Social Emotional Risk Factors

  • Published:
Child Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Currently most leading authors in empirical developmental research agree that cognitive, emotional and social development are closely connected from the very start. Many researchers share a transactional understanding of development which means that particularly the social-emotional development always takes place in the interaction of an individual with his personal and nonpersonal environment. As will be discussed in this paper: many authors postulate that during evolution only human beings have developed a specialized inferential and representational system for investigating mental states (see e.g. the so called „Theory of Mind“ (ToM) or mindreading). On the other hand human beings are the only one of the social species with an attachment – instinct system for whom the early care of the infant is essential for developing adaptive functioning in social relationships. Empirical findings of different disciplines indicate that attachment and mentalizing are two independent adaptive achievements which have been developed by selection and which are essential for different functions but are dependent on each other during early development. Considering these two developmental lines risk and protective factors of the social emotional development will be elaborated. Finally some possibilities to strengthen the resiliency and the development of prosocial behaviour of „children-at-risk “in different forms of prevention in Kindergartens and schools will be discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The IDeA Center is a large interdisciplinary institution for studying „children-at-risk“. The German Institute for Educational Research (Dipf), the Johann-Wolfgang-Goethe University and the Sigmund-Freud-Institut, Frankfurt a. M., are the cooperating institutions. The center is financially supported by the Landes-Offensive zur Entwicklung Wissenschaftlich-ökonomischer Exzellenz (LOEWE) of the State of Hessen, Germany. Currentl around 120 scientists from many different disciplines (educational sciences, psychology, psychoanalysis, neurosciences, didactics etc.) are engaged in 25 different projects in the center.

  2. A further problematic dynamic can also be enforced by a mismatch of temperament and idiosyncratic neurobiological rhythms of the infant and the primary object (see II). For example: Ellman (2010) most impressively pointed out the consequences the adequate and inadequate primary care-giver’s interpretations of sleep-rhythms and the infant’s temperament can have. A temperamental infant can be exposed to over-stimulation (going as far as sleeplessness) and fall into an unbearable psycho-physiological state through an over-stimulating primary object. It is in need of a care giver who is able to prevent over-stimulation. – In contrast infants with a more lethargic temperament require adequate stimulation in order to experience a lustful inner state, which is necessary for a sufficiently good activation during the wake-periods, which then again facilitate recreational sleep. – Weinstein and Ellman (2012) and the other researchers were able to show, which problematic psycho-physiological consequences the care giver’s misinterpretation of the infant’s idiosyncratic and mostly genetically designated behaviour can have. According to Fonagy and Luyten (2011) such early and in extreme cases even traumatizing interaction experiences can lead to a inhibition or detachment of social cognition or in other words the collapse of mentalization. The child’s self is not given the possibility to develop an age appropriate psychic equivalence-mode through an attachment figure’s constant, “understandable” and consistent anticipatable actions and subsequent adequate affect-regulation. The child is therefore neither capable of developing an adequate affect-regulation nor the basic roots for mentalization. Further developmental states of mentalization are compromised or even prevented by such attachment experiences. The authors refer to a “hyperactive attachment-system” (see centre of graph).

  3. Within the frame of this paper we have to focus on some studies from the field of attachment research. Similar findings are reported from studies on the development to mentalize .

References

  • Ahnert, L., & Harwardt, E. (2008). Die beziehungserfahrungen der vorschulzeit und ihre bedeutung für den schuleintritt. Empirische Pädagogik, 22, 145–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ammaniti, M., Trentini, C., Menozzi, F. & Tambelli, R. (in press): Transition to parenthood between mind and brain. Will be published in Emde, R. & Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. (eds.): Early parenting and the prevention of disorders. Interdisciplinary research. London: Karnac

  • Atashrouz, B., Pakdaman, S. H., & Asgari, A. (2008). Prediction of academic achievement from attachment rate. Journal of Family Research Summer, 4, 193–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aviezer, O., Sagi, A., Resnick, G., & Gini, M. (2002). School competence in young adolescence: Links to early attachment relationships beyond concurrent selfperceived competence and representations of relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 397–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B. & Lachmann, F. (2002). Infant Research and Adult Treatment: Co-constructing Interactions. London: Analytic Press.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 5–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., & Fearon, P. (2002). Early attachment security, subsequent maternal sensitivity, and later child development: Does continuity in development depend upon continuity of caregiving? Attachment and Human Development, 4, 361–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Ravine, M., & Taylor, D. G. (1984). The pennsylvania infant and family development project, III: The origins of individual differences in infant-mother attachment: Maternal and infant contributions. Child Development, 55, 718–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bengel, J., Meinders-Lücking, F. & Rottmann, N. (2009): Schutzfaktoren bei Kindern und Jugendlichen – Stand der Forschung zu psychosozialen Schutzfaktoren für Gesundheit (Forschung und Praxis der Gesundheitsförderung, Bd. 35). Köln: Bundeszentrale für gesundheitliche Aufklärung.

  • Berger,S., Jurist, E.,Slade, A.(eds) (2008) Mind to mind: Infant research, neuroscience and psychoanalysis: Mentalization, internalization and representation. NY:Other Press.

  • Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the classroom. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 141–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohleber, W. (2010). Destructiveness, intersubjectivity, and trauma. The identity crises of modern psychoanalysis. London: Karnac.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Rydell, A.-M. (2000). Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal study from infancy to middle childhood. Social Development, 9, 25–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosmans, G., Braet, C., & Van Vlierberghe, L. (2010). Attachment and symptoms of psychopathology: Early maladaptive schemas as a cognitive link? Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 17, 374–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Bindung. Eine Analyse der Mutter-Kind-Beziehung. München: Kindler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (1874). Psychology from an empirical standpoint. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1985): Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. In: Bretherton, I. & Waters, F. (eds.): Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209): 3–35.

  • Brisch, K.-H. & Hellbruegge, T. (2009): Wege zu sicheren Bindungen in Familie und Gesellschaft. Prävention, Begleitung, Beratung und Psychotherapie. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.

  • Brüser, E. (2011). Sinn und Unsinn früher Förderung - Im Blick der Öffentlichkeit. In H. Keller (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 1176–1193). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burger, K. (2010). How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive development? An international review of the effects of early interventions for children from different social backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 140–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camilli, G., Vargas, S., Ryan, S., & Barnett, W. S. (2010). Meta-Analysis of the effects of early education interventions on cognitive and social development. Teachers College Record Volume, 112, 579–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed.). London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Champagne, F. A. (2008). Epigenetic mechanisms and the transgenerational effects of maternal care. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 29, 386–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cierpka, M. (2004). Faustlos. Ein Curriculum zur Förderung sozial-emotionaler Kompetenzen und zur Gewaltprävention bei Kindern. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cierpka, M., Scholtes, K., Frey, B. & Koehler, H. (2011): Der Übergang zur Elternschaft. In: Keller, H. & Rümmele, A. (Hg.): Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung. Bern: Huber,1090–1109.

  • Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Internal representational models of attachment relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 259–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Rosnay, M., & Harris, P. L. (2002). Individual differences in children’s understanding of emotion: The roles of attachment and language. Attachment and Human Development, 4, 39–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behaviour and Brain Sciences, 4, 568–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeWolff, M. S., & Van Ijzerdoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dozier, M., Stovall, K. C., Albus, K. E., & Bates, B. (2001). Attachment for infants in foster care: The role of caregiver state of mind. Child Development, 72, 1467–1477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egle, U. T., Hoffmann, S. O., & Joraschky, P. (Eds.). (2000). Sexueller Mißbrauch, Mißhandlung, Vernachlässigung. Erkennung und Therapie psychischer und psychosomatischer Folgen früher Traumatisierungen. 2, vollst. aktualis. u. erw. Aufl. Stuttgart/New York: Schattauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellman, S. (2010). When theories touch: A historical and theoretical integration of psychoanalytic thought. London: Karnac.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R. N. (2011). Regeneration und neuanfänge. Perspektiven einer entwicklungsbezogenen ausrichtung der psychoanalyse. Psyche – Z Psychoana, 65, 778–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R.N., Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. (eds)(in press): Early parenting research and prevention of disorder: Psychoanalytic research at intrdisciplinary frontiers. London: Karnac books

  • Fodor, J. A. (1992). A theory of child’s theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P. (2007): Violent attachment. Vortrag auf der Tagung “In Gewalt verstrickt – psychoanalytische, pädagogische und philosophische Erkundungen“, Universität Kassel, 9./10.2.2007.

  • Fonagy, P. (2010). Veränderungen der klinischen praxis: Wissenschaftlich oder pragmatisch begründet? In K. Münch et al. (Eds.), Die Psychoanalyse im Pluralismus der Wissenschaften (pp. 33–81). Gießen: Psychosozial-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2011). Die entwicklungspsychologischen wurzeln der borderline-persönlichkeitsstörung in kindheit und adoleszenz: ein forschungsbericht unter dem blickwinkel der mentalisierungstheorie. Psyche - Z Psychoanal, 65, 900–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2003a). Psychoanalyse und die psychopathologie der entwicklung. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.

  • Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2003b). Psychoanalytic theories-perspectives from developmental psychopathology. New York: Brunner-Routledge.

  • Friedrich, L. & Siegert, M. (2009). Förderung des Bildungserfolgs von Migranten: Effekte familienorientierter Projekte. Abschlussbericht zum Projekt: Bildungserfolge bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Migrationshintergrund durch Zusammenarbeit mit den Eltern, Working Paper 24 des BAMF.

  • Gergely, G., & Unoka, Z. (2008). Attachment and mentalization in humans. The development of the affective self. In E. L. Jurist, A. Slade, & S. Bergner (Eds.), Mind to mind. Infant research, neuroscience, and psychoanalysis (pp. 50–87). New York: Other Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geserick, B. (2004): Die Bedeutung von Bindung und mütterlicher Unterstützung für die emotionale Regulation von sechsjährigen Kindern in Anforderungssituationen. The role of attachment and material support for the emotional regulation of 6-year-old children in strain situations. Universität Gießen, Fachbereich Psychologie und Sportwissenschaft, Diss.

  • Gloger-Tippelt, G. (Ed.). (2011). Bindung im erwachsenenalter. ein handbuch für forschung und praxis. (2. Aufl.). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, J., Stanley, C., Smith, V., & Goldwyn, R. (2000). A new method of evaluating attachment representations in young school-age children: The manchester child attachment story task. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 48–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1991). Attachment quality as an organizer of emotional and behavioral responses in a longitudinal perspective. In C. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & M. Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 234–267). London/New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affective social competence. Social Development, 10, 79–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 1, pp. 75–97). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, S. T., Allen, J.-P., & Golden, E. (2006). Out of the woods. Tales of resilient teens. London: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J. (2008). Early childhood education and care: The case for investing in disadvantaged young children. CESifo DICE Report, 6(2), 3–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobsen, T., & Hoffman, V. (1997). Children’s attachment representations: Longitudinal relations to school behavior and academic competency in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 33, 703–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, P. S., El-Sheikh, M., & Buckhalt, J. A. (2008). Children’s attachment to parents and their academic functioning: Sleep disruptions as moderators of effects. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 29, 441–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerns, K. (2008). Attachment in middle childhood. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment. Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 366–382). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Interdisziplinäre Arbeitsgemeinschaft Kindeswohl – Kindeswohlgefährdung (2002): Die Methode „Gemeinsam helfen – wenn das Kindeswohl eines Säuglings gefährdet ist. “Hannover, Leitfaden.

  • Koglin, U., & Petermann, F. (2006). Verhaltenstraining im kindergarten: ein programm zur förderung sozial-emotionaler kompetenz. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1994). Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of TOMM. Cognition, 50, 211–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. (2009). Frühe kindheit als schicksal? trauma, embodiment. Kohlhammer: Soziale Desintegration. Psychoanalytische Perspektiven. Stuttgart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leuzinger-Bohleber, M., & Burkhardt-Mußmann, C. (2012): Sexueller Missbrauch: ein Trauma mit lebenslangen Folgen. In: Thole, W.; Baader, M.; Helsper, W.; Kappeler, M.; Leuzinger-Bohleber, M.; Reh, S.; Sielert, U.; Thompson, Ch. (Hg.): Sexualisierte Gewalt, Macht und Pädagogik. (Publikation der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Erziehungswissenschaft, DGfE). Opladen [u.a.]: Budrich, 186–207.

  • Leuzinger-Bohleber, M., & Pfeifer, R. (2002). Remembering a depressive primary object? memory in the dialogue between psychoanalysis and cognitive science. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 83, 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuzinger-Bohleber, M., Canestri, J., & Target, M. (Eds.). (2010). Early development and its disturbances: Clinical, conceptual and empirical research on ADHD and other psychopathologies and its epistemological reflections (pp. 185–206). London: Karnac Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leuzinger-Bohleber, M., Läzer, K. L., Pfenning-Meerkötter, N., Fischmann, T., Wolff, A., & Green, J. (2011). Psychoanalytic treatment of ADHD children in the frame of two extraclinical studies: The frankfurt prevention study and the eva study. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 10, 32–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebermann, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lohaus, A., Korntheurer, P., & Lissmann, I. (2010). Bindung und die entwicklung von sprache und kognition als voraussetzungen für den bildungserwerb. In C. Spiel et al. (Eds.), Bildungspsychologie (pp. 40–44). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Development, 64, 572–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Cibelli, C. D. (1997). Infant attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology, 33, 681–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Atwood, G. (1999). A relational diathesis model of hostile-helpless states of mind. In J. Solomon & C. Goerge (Eds.), Attachment disorganization (pp. 33–70). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, J., & Nelson, G. (2000). Programs for the promotion of family wellness and the prevention of child maltreatment: A meta-analytic review. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 1127–1149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madigan, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., Van IJzendoorn, M., Moran, G., Pederson, D., & Benoit, D. (2006). Unresolved states of mind, anomalous parental behaviour, and disorganized attachment: A review and meta-analysis of a transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 89–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: Recent studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Human Development, 33, 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2), 66–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marées, N. V., & Petermann, F. (2009). Förderung sozial-emotionaler kompetenzen im grundschulalter. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 244–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayes, R. (2012). The neurobiology of parenting and attachment: The interface between the psychology and physiology of caring. vortrag auf der joseph sandler psychoanalytic research conference. In A. M. Frankfurt, R. Emde, & M. Leuzinger-Bohleber (Eds.), Early parenting and the prevention of disorders. Interdisciplinary research (pp. 129–157). London: Karnac.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., & Russell, J. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., et al. (2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 1194–1211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melhuish, K. S., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (Eds.). (2010). Early childhood matters. Evidence from the effective pre-school and primary education project. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, E., & St-Laurent, D. (2001). Attachment at school age and academic performance. Developmental Psychology, 37, 863–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, E., Cyr, C., & Dubois-Comtois, K. (2004). Attachment at early school age and developmental risk: Examining family contexts and behavior problems of controlling-caregiving, controlling-punitive, and behaviorally disorganized children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 519–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, E., Smolla, N., Cyr, C., Dubois-Comtois, K., Mazzarello, T., & Berthiaume, C. (2006). Attachment and behavior problems in middle childhood as reported by adult and child informants. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 425–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nathanson, D. (1986). The empathetic wall and the ecology of affect. The Psychoanalytic Study oft he Child, 41, 171–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nore, M., & Barnett, W. S. (2010). Benefits of early childhood interventions across the world: (Under) investing in the very young. Economics of Education Review, 29, 271–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, E., & McCartney, K. (2007). Examining teacher–child relationships and achievement as part of an ecological model of development. American Educational Research Journal, 44, 340–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2008). Programs for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 355–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ontai, L. L., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). Patterns of attachment and maternal discourse effects on children’s emotion understanding from 3- to 5-years of age. Social Development, 11, 433–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, D., Emde, R. N., Hasson, M., & Warren, S. (1997). Preschoolers face moral dilemmas: A longitudinal study of acknowledgment and resolving internal conflict. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 78, 943.957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, R. M., Heron, J., Melotti, R., Joinson, C., Stein, A., Ramchandandi, P. G., & Evans, J. (2011). The association between observed non-verbal maternal response at 12 months and later infant development at 18 months and IQ at 4 years: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior & Development, 34, 525–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petermann, F.& Wiedebusch, S. (2008): Emotionale Kompetenz bei Kindern (2., überarb. und erw. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  • Petermann, F., & Kullik, A. (2011). Frühe Emotionsdysregulation: Ein Indikator für psychische Störungen im Kindesalter? Kindheit und Entwicklung, 20, 186–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petermann, F., & Wiedebusch, S. (2002). Störungen beim Erwerb emotionaler Kompetenz im Kindesalter. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 50, 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raikes, H.H. & Emde, R. (2006): Early head start: A bold new program for low-income infants and toddlers. In: Watt, N.F. et al. (eds.): The crisis in youth mental health: Critical issues and effective programs. (Vol. 4: Early intervention programs and policies). Westport: Praeger Press, 181–206.

  • Reichle, B., & Gloger-Tippelt, G. (2007). Familiale kontexte und sozial-emotionale entwicklung. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 16, 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saami, C. (2002). Die Entwicklung von emotionaler Kompetenz in Beziehungen. In M. V. Salisch (Ed.), Emotionale kompetenz entwickeln. Grundlagen in kindheit und jugend (pp. 3–30). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J. (1989). Models of developmental regulation: the environtype. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Rochester Symposium on Development Psychopathology (Vol. 1, pp. 41–68). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schechter, D. (2014). Understanding how traumatized mothers process their toddlers’ affective communication under stress: Towards preventive intervention for families at high risk for intergenerational violence. Vortrag auf der Joseph Sandler Psychoanalytic Research Conference. In A. M. Frankfurt, R. Emde, & M. Leuzinger-Bohleber (Eds.), Early parenting and the prevention of disorders. Interdisciplinary research (pp. 238–264). London: Karnac.

  • Schechter, D. S., Myers, M. M., Brunelli, S. A., Coates, S. W., Zeanahm, C. H., Davies, M., Grienenberger, J. F., Marshall, R. D., McCaw, J. E., Trabka, K. A., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2006). Traumatized mothers can change their minds about their toddlers: Understanding how a novel use of videofeedback supports positive change of maternal attributions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27, 429–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schleiffer, R., et al. (2009). Konsequenzen unsicherer bindungsqualität: Verhaltensauffälligkeiten und schulleistungsprobleme. In J. Henri (Ed.), Bindung im Kindesalter. Diagnostik und Intervention (pp. 39–63). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment and Human Development, 7, 283–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B., & Ziegenhain, U. (2012). Die normale psychische entwicklung und ihre varianten. In J. M. Fegert, C. H. Eggers, & F. Resch (Eds.), Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters. (2. Aufl.) (pp. 35–59). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment security in infancy and childhood. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 287–318). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, J., George, C., & De Jong, A. (1995). Children classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and at school. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 447–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spieker, S. J., Nelson, D. C., Petras, A., Jolley, S. N., & Barnard, K. E. (2003). Joint influence of child care and infant attachment security for cognitive and language outcomes of low-income toddlers. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 326–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363–1373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stacks, A. M., & Oshio, T. (2009). Disorganized attachment and social skills as indicators of head start childrens’s school readiness skills. Attachment and Human Development, 11, 143–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stams, G., Juffer, F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2002). Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental Psychology, 38, 806–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1985). Die lebenserfahrung des säuglings. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suomi, S. (2010). Trauma and epigenetics. Vortrag bei der 11th Joseph Sandler Research Conference 2010: “Persisting Shadows of Early and Later Trauma”. Frankfurt am Main, 7.2.2010

  • Tellings, A. (1999). Psychoanalytical and genetic-structuralistic approaches of moral development. Psychoanalytic Review, 86, 903–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teti, D. M. (1999). Conceptualizations of disorganization in the preschool years: An integration. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disorganization (pp. 213–242). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. A. (2008). Measure twice, cut once: Attachment theory and the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Attachment & Human Development, 10, 287–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Harvard University Press.

  • Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., Sacco, F. C., Vernberg, E., & Malcom, J. M. (2011). Reducing violence and prejudice in a Jamaican all age school using attachment and mentalization theory. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28, 497–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Ijzendoorn, M.H., Kroonenberg, P.M.(1988): Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the Strange Situation. Child Development, 59,147–156

  • van Ijzendoorn, M.H. & Sagi, A. (2008): Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In: Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.): Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford, 880–905

  • Wadepohl, H., Koglin, U., Vonderlin, E., & Petermann, F. (2011). Förderung sozial-emotionaler kompetenz im kindergarten Evaluation eines präventiven Verhaltenstrainings. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 20, 219–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinert, S., & Weinert, F. E. (2006). Entwicklung der entwicklungspsychologie: Wurzeln, Meilensteine, Entwicklungslinien. In W. Schneider & F. Wilkening (Eds.), Theorien, modelle und methoden der entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 3–58). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinfeld, N., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. A. (1999). The nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment. Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 68–88). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, L., & Ellman, S. (2012). Die bedeutung der endogenen stimulation für das träumen und die entwicklung. Psyche- Z Psychoanal, 66, 862–888.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marianne Leuzinger-Bohleber.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. Social Emotional Risk Factors. Child Ind Res 7, 715–734 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9261-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9261-7

Keywords

Navigation