Skip to main content
Log in

Explicating Individual Training Decisions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Vocations and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we explicate individual training decisions. For this purpose, we propose a framework based on instrumentality theory, a psychological theory of motivation that has frequently been applied to individual occupational behavior. To test this framework, we employ novel German individual data and estimate the effect of subjective expected utility (SEU) from continuing vocational training (CVT), the effect of restricting factors, and the effect of personal characteristics on the willingness to pay for CVT. Our results imply that SEU is generally the main driver of training decisions. In contrast, financial restrictions are most decisive for persons who are more likely to participate in training (i.e., training tendency). Time restraints also help explain why some individuals are entirely unwilling to participate. Moreover, regional infrastructure is a crucial training determinant. We also find that age and vocational qualifications do not directly affect training decisions. However, persons in specific occupational settings (e.g., low occupational status, overly challenging workplace situations) do exhibit a lower training tendency. Additionally, the training behavior of these persons appears to be more rigid, making them less likely to react to changes in their cognitive attitudes to training.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. When speaking about individual training ‘decisions’ or training ‘participation’, we actually refer to the decisions (respectively tendencies or motivation) of individuals to contribute to the financing of CVT; we do not refer to the decisions of individuals to actually participate. This is, because in many cases participation additionally depends on employers’ decisions. Of course, actual participation can be further boosted, if employers contribute to the financing in case they have an own interest in training their employees. Nevertheless, we assume individual training tendency (and thus the probability of participation) to be higher when willingness to pay is higher. In this paper, we are interested only in explicating individual training motivation, not in explicating the training motivation of employers. For better readability we nevertheless decided to stick to the terms ‘individual training decision’ and’individual training participation’, being aware that employers influence participation, too.

  2. We exclude unemployed persons, whose training decisions may be based on an entirely different subset of valences and restrictions. An integrated investigation of the training decisions of employed and unemployed would require an operationalization of the theory, which explicitly considers these different valences and restrictions, making the empirical effort much more complex.

  3. The Determinants of Individual Continuing Training (DICT) Survey 2010 was commissioned by the German Federal Institute for Vocational Education and Training (BIBB) and pursued through forsa GmbH seated in Berlin (Germany) from September through October 2010.

  4. An omnibus survey was used to identify persons willing to participate in the main survey. The given response rate represents the number of completed interviews (1,600) divided by the number of persons expressing willingness to participate in the main survey during the omnibus survey (2,514). It is not known, however, how many persons the surveying firm contacted to find 2,514 persons who were willing to participate in the main interview. Hence, the true response rate is unknown.

  5. For the purpose of SEU computation, the ordinal four-point scale was numerically interpreted, and the integer values 0–3 were assigned to the four categories.

  6. For the purpose of SEU computation, the ordinal four-point scale was numerically interpreted, and the values 0, 0.33, 0.66, and 1 were assigned to the four categories.

  7. We conducted all statistical analyses with the help of the statistical software package Stata 11. Partial proportional odds models were fitted using the Stata GOLOGIT module.

References

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Backes-Gellner, U., Mure, J., & Tuor, S. (2007). The Puzzle of Non-Participation in Continuing Training – An empirical Study of Chronic vs. Temporary Non-Participation. Zeitschrift für Arbeitsmarktforschung, 40(2–3), 295–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behringer, F., & Descamps, R. (2009). Determinants of employer-provided training: A comparative analysis of Germany and France. In F. Behringer, B. Käpplinger, & G. Pätzold (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Berufs- und Wirtschaftspädagogik. Beiheft 22 (pp. 94–123). Stuttgart: Franz Steiner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J.-L., & D’Ascoli, Y. (2012). Motivations to Become Vocational Education and Training Educators: A Person-Oriented Approach. Vocations and Learning, 5(3), 225–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blundell, R., Dearden, L., Meghir, C., & Sianesi, B. (1999). Human Capital Investment: The Returns from Education and Training to the Individual, the Firm and the Economy. Fiscal Studies, 20(1), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brant, R. (1990). Assessing Proportionality in the Proportional Odds Model for Ordinal Logistic Regression. Biometrics, 46(4), 1171–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descy, P., & Tessaring, M. (2005). The value of learning: evaluation and impact of education and training. Luxemburg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities

  • Eerde, W. V., & Thierry, H. (1996). Vroom’s Expectancy Models and Work-Related Criteria: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 575–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple Processes by which Attitudes Guide Behavior: The MODE Model as an Integrative Framework. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23(1), 75–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fourage, D., Schils, T., & de Grip, A. (2013). Why Do Low-Educated Workers Invest Less in Further Training? Applied Economics, 45(18), 2587–2601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabay-Egozi, L., Shavit, Y., & Yaish, M. (2010). Curricular Choice: A Test of a Rational Choice Model of Education. European Sociological Review, 26(4), 447–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galbraith, J., & Cummings, L. (1967). An Empirical Investigation of the Motivational Determinants of Task Performance: Interactive Effects between Instrumentality-Valence and Motivation-Ability. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 237–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gegenfurtner, A. (2013). Dimensions of Motivation to Transfer: A Longitudinal Analysis of Their Influence on Retention, Transfer, and Attitude Change. Vocations and Learning, 6(2), 187–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, B., Mahoney, G., & Jones, N. (1957). A Path-Goal Approach to Productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 41(6), 345–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, W., & Wolfe, R. (1998). An approximate likelihood-ratio test for ordinal response models. Stata Technical Bulletin, 42(March 1998), 24–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gritz, R. (1993). The impact of training on the frequency and duration of employment. Journal of Econometrics, 57(1), 21–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. (2006). Positive Externalities and Government Involvement in Education. Journal of Private Enterprise, 22(2), 165–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jodlbauer, S., Selenko, E., Batinic, B., & Stiglbauer, B. (2011). The relationship between job dissatisfaction and training transfer. International Journal of Training and Development, 16(1), 39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinicki, A. (1989). Predicting Occupational Choices after Involuntary Job Loss. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 35(2), 204–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyndt, E., Govaerts, N., Dochy, F., & Baert, H. (2011). The Learning Intention of Low-Qualified Employees: A Key for Participation in Lifelong Learning and Continuous Training. Vocations and Learning, 4(3), 211–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, E., & Suttle, J. (1973). Expectancy Theory and Job Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 9, 482–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuven, E. (2005). The Economics of Private Sector Training: A Survey of the Literature. Journal of Economic Surveys, 19(1), 91–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, M., & Spletzer, J. (1999). General and Specific Training: Evidence and Implications. The Journal of Human Resources, 34(4), 710–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, L. (1991). The Role of Off-the-Job vs. On-the-Job Training for the Mobility of Women Workers. The American Economic Review, 81(2), 151–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miner, J. (2003). The Rated Importance, Scientific Validity, and Practical Usefulness of Organizational Behavior Theories: A Quantitative Review. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 2(3), 250–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, T. (1974). Expectancy Models of Job Satisfaction, Occupational Preference and Effort: A Theoretical, Methodological, and Empirical Appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81(12), 1053–1077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, T., & Albright, D. (1972). Expectancy Theory Predictions of the Satisfaction, Effort, Performance and Retention of Naval Aviation Officers. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2011). Education at a Glance 2011 – OECD Indicators. Paris: OECD

  • Offerhaus, J. (2013). The type to train? Impacts of personality characteristics on further training participation. SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research, (531–2013).

  • Osborn, D. (1990). A Reexamination of the Organizational Choice Process. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 36(1), 45–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and flow. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge: University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1959). Theories of Decision-Making in Economics and Behavioral Science. The American Economic Review, 49(3), 253–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., & West, R. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 23, 645–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations (2001). World Population Ageing: 1950–2050. New York: United Nations Publications

  • Vroom, V. (1964). Work and Motivation. New York: Wiley Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vroom, V. (2005). On the origins of expectancy theory. In K. Smith & M. Hitt (Eds.), Great Minds in Management: The Process of Theory Development (pp. 239–258). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wahba, M., & House, R. (1974). Expectancy Theory in Work and Motivation: Some Logical and Methodological Issues. Human Relations, 27(2), 121–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, M., & Müller, N. (2013). Determinants of Individual Continuing Training 2010 (BIBB-DICT-Survey 2010). suf_1.0. Bonn: Federal Institute for Vocational Education and Training. doi: 10.7803/611.10.1.1.10

  • Williams, R. (2006). Generalized ordered Logit/ Partial Proportial Odds Models for Ordinal Dependent Variables. The Stata Journal, 61(1), 58–82.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcel Walter.

Appendix

Appendix

Composition of Subjective Expected Utility: Algebraic formulation

$$ SEU=\left({E}_j\times \left[{\displaystyle \sum_k\left({V}_k\times {I}_{jk}\right)}\right]\right)\left({\displaystyle \sum_l\left({V}_l\times {I}_l\right)}\right) $$
(1)

At first, all valences V were weighted by their respecting instrumentalities I. Then the individual IxV-weights of the extrinsic outcomes were multiplied with the mean measure of all three expectancy values. Note that, in contrast to extrinsic goals, intrinsic outcomes are directly linked to the action (i.e., CVT participation) itself. Thus, the sum of the products Vl × Il immediately represents the observed individuals’ intrinsic motivation for CVT participation, whereas the sum of the products Vk × Ijk merely represents the valence of the actions’ first-order outcome (i.e., training success). Weighted also with the respective expectancy value, Vk × Ijk represents an individuals’ extrinsic motivation for CVT. The multiplicative combination of valences, instrumentalities, and expectations implies that actions are attractive only when personal goals are rated relatively high and when the respective action is simultaneously perceived to foster the attainment of highly rated goals. However, additive combinations between variables have also been adopted in the empirical literature (Wahba and House 1974).

The summarization and multiplication of instrumentalities, expectancies and values has been criticized as being overly deterministic. Following a common criticism, individuals do not calculate SEU-scores for different actions before they take an action. Indeed, theories of rational action do not postulate that individuals multiply expectancies and values for various potential actions. The theories rather assume that individuals choose, among alternatives, those actions, which lead to both desirable and achievable goals. Multiplication of V, I, and E measures is simply the method used in empirical investigations to approximate this behavior.

Nevertheless, those calculations have been criticized for another reason: when summarizing and multiplying the measures of expectancy, instrumentality and valence, one would disregard the limitations of the scales used (Mitchell 1974; Vroom 2005).

Formal regression model to test hypotheses 1–3:

$$ \ln \frac{P\left(WTP>m\Big|x\right)}{P\left(WTP\le m\Big|x\right)}={\beta}_o+{\beta}_{SEUm}SEU+{\beta}_{RESTRm}RESTR+{\beta}_{PERSm}PERS+\varepsilon, m=1,2,\dots, 6 $$
(2)

The left-hand side of the equation specifies the logarithmic odds in favor of willingness to pay being greater than the threshold level m (i.e., the ratio of the probability that the willingness to pay (WTP) is greater than m to the probability that willingness to pay is less than or equal to m), given the values of the predictors. The right-hand side of the equation includes the SEU indicator, a set of restrictions (RESTR), and a vector of personal characteristics (PERS).

Running an approximate LR test (Gould and Wolfe 1998) and a Wald test (Brant 1990) indicates that the parallel lines assumption only holds for 15 variables in our specified model. Therefore, we use a partial proportional odds model (Williams 2006) for estimation.

Formal regression model to test hypotheses 4 and 5:

$$ \ln \frac{P\left(WTP>\left.m\right|x\right)}{P\left(WTP\le \left.m\right|x\right)}={\beta}_o+{\beta}_{SEU}SEU+{\beta}_{RESTR} RESTR+{\beta}_{PERS} PERS+{\beta}_{PERS\times SEU} PERS\times SEU+\varepsilon $$
(3)

“Imagine you could participate in a continuing training course. The course is about [SUBJECT]. In particular, this may be [TOPIC]. The course [SCOPE] and you would visit it outside of your regular working schedule on weekdays and/or on weekends.”

 

SUBJECT

TOPIC

SCOPE

Scenario 1

extending your qualification and enabling you to lead other employees

Case sensitive a

duration accounts for about 800 h, e.g., 100 work days

Scenario 2

computer software

, dependent on your background knowledge, a beginners, advanced or expert course, for example word processing, e-mail, Computer Aided Design etc., which is used in your professional field

 

Scenario 3

improving your social skills

a course about taking speeches, paralanguage, presenting, communicating, negotiating, team working or coping with stress

duration accounts for 16 h

Scenario 4

expanding knowledge in your occupational field

any course about technical aspects in your occupational field

  1. aThe interviewers gave examples dependent on the employment status and qualification of the employees. For example, if the employee had been a craftsman without tertiary degree, the TOPIC would have been: a course which prepares you for the master craftsman’s examination.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Walter, M., Mueller, N. Explicating Individual Training Decisions. Vocations and Learning 8, 159–183 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12186-014-9127-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12186-014-9127-7

Keywords

Navigation