Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural Psychology of Differences and EMS; a New Theoretical Framework for Understanding and Reconstructing Culture

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper I introduce the outlines of our new type of theoretical framework named ‘Cultural psychology of Differences’ for understanding cultural others and dialogically reconstructing interactions among cultural others. In order to understand cultural others, it is necessary for us to reconstruct a new concept which enables us to analyze dynamic generation processes of culture. We propose the concept of Expanded Mediational Structure, EMS, as an elementary unit for understanding human social interactions. EMS is composed of subjects who interacts each other using objects of some kind as mediators, and a normative mediator, NM, which mediates their interactions. It is necessary to generate, share and adjust a NM to keep social interactions stable, and culture will appear when interaction malfunction is attributed to a gaps of NMs. The concept of EMS helps us to understand how culture is functionally substantialized in the plane of collective (or communal) intersubjectivity and how cultural conflicts develop and intensify. Focusing on the generation process of culture through interactions provides us with another option to understand cultural others through dialogical interactions with them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We can differentiate intersubjectivity into two levels. The first level is dyadic intersubjectivity composed of two subjects who are assumed to share their subjective world. We can interpret that interactions between these subjects are based on their mutual intersubjective understanding. The second one is triadic intersubjectivity which is composed of three subjects including a third party. When the two subjects interact with each other with an awareness of the third party, we can assume their interactions have social nature. The third party herein can be a person like a mother to a child, a certain rule shared by subjects, or a law in social interaction. All of these influence interacting subjects and function as the normative element for them. We name the latter collective (or communal) intersubjectivity (共同主観性 kyodoshukansei in Japanese: see also Hiromatsu, W. 1972). This kind of subjectivity is the fundamental base of human social interactions which construct human social system.

  2. The following discussion is the summary of Yamamoto (2015) which has been elaborated as relatively independent of the traditional theories of psychology about culture. Therefore, the discussion might be able to understand more easily by taking notice of the theoretical composition itself than by seeking the backgrounds of it in other traditional psychological theories.

  3. Besides, culture is not arbitral in the sense that no one can freely choose his/her origin and circumstances for his/ her development. For example, toddlers’ interactions over possession begin to change around the age of one and a half year (Yamamoto 1991a). The dominant patterns of possessional behaviors observed among children around the age of two differ between Japanese and Chinese children, which agrees with the difference in pattern of cultural possessional behaviors between Japanese and Chinese adults (Yamamoto 1997). Since possession is an essential component of self (cf. the notion of material me-- James 1907), these developmental facts indicate that their selves begin to be culturally organized by the age of two at least. A person’s life is oriented by the nature of his/ her self, but no one can choose it arbitrarily.

  4. Therefore, we have to discriminate two levels of intersubjectivity as foot note 2.

  5. This kind of relative objectivity of NM is not the objectivity of natural science which considers a physical object as being out of human subjectivity. But, in the sense that natural science itself is a part of human intellectual activity in plane of collective-intersubjectivity, it can be considered as a specific case of such objectivity. (cf. Hiromatsu 1972).

  6. Lévi-Strauss also mentions that his structural interpretation of myths is not the only one, and it will appear differently to the others (Lévi-Strauss 1978). He also claims that the relativity of interpretation reflects the nature of culture (Lévi-Strauss 1964). Hence his objectivism seen in his criticism to Mauss doesn’t seem to be based on simple dualism of objectivity and subjectivity, and we share such kind of theoretical viewpoint with him as far as this point. In "Formation of Static Stereotypical Understanding of the Cultural Other" section, we already mentioned the ambiguity of culture based on the nature of culture generation which derives from collective intersubjectivity of the culture.

References

  • Asad, T. (1986). The concept of cultural translation in British social anthropology. In E. J. Cliffrod & G. E. Marcus (Eds.), Writing culture; the poetics and politics of ethnography (pp. 141–164). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chevalier, J. M., & Buckles, D. J. (2013). Participatory action research: Theory and methods for engaged inquiry. Oxford: Routledge.

  • Cole, M. (1998). Cultural psychology, a once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity-theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki: Oriental Konsultit Oy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiromatsu, W. (1972). Collective subjective structure of the world. Tokyo: Keiso Shobo [in Japanese].

  • Ishishita, K., Mizuguchi, K., Watanabe, T. & Yang, J. (2012). Cultural understanding between Chinese and Japanese high school students through the dialogical cultural exchange class. Proceedings for the 10th anniversary of the Japan association of Chinese language education and the 30th anniversary of the association of high school Chinese language education, 39–42. [in Japanese]

  • James, W. (1907). Psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1950). Introduction à l'oeuvre de Marcel Mauss, in Mauss, M. 1950 Sociologie et Anthropologie. Paris: Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1964). Le Cru et Le Cuit, Mythologiques. Paris: PLON.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1978). Myth and meaning: Five talks for radio by Claude Lévi-Strauss. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and psychology; people around the world. California: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, K. (1911). A study of good. Tokyo: Kodohkan [in Japanese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Oh, S. (2016a). The structure of relationships of friends mediated by money. In N. Takahashi & T. Yamamoto (Eds.), Money as a cultural tool for East Asian children: Cultural developmental psychology of “pocket money” (pp. 73–95). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press [in Japanese].

  • Oh, S. (2016b). When cultural difference appears and overcomes. In N. Takahashi & T. Yamamoto (Eds.), Money as a cultural tool for East Asian children: Cultural developmental psychology of “pocket money” (pp. 213–240). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press [in Japanese].

  • Oh, S. (2017). Dialogical exchange class using movies for mutual understanding between a Korean and a Japanese Universities. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 51(3).

  • Oh, S., Pian, C., Yamamoto, T., Takahashi, N., Sato, T., Taleo, K., Choi, S., & Kim, S. (2005). Money and the life worlds of children in Korea: Examing the phenomenon of Ogori (treating) from cultural psychological perspectives. Maebashi Kyoai Gakuen College Ronshu, 5, 73–88.

  • Oh, S., Choi, S. & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Group Inter-cultural Understanding (1) Through the “Round-Table Cinema” Classes between Japanese and Korean Universities, Maebashi Kyoai Gakuen College Ronshu, 14, 127–143

  • Pian, C. (2017). What happened in dialogical classes of intercultural understanding?: an analysis of exchanging classes between Chinese and Japanese university students. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 51 (3).

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1985). Language acquisition as increasing linguistic structuring of experience and symbolic behavior control. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative: Towards a theory of language and mind. Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakakibara, T. (2017). Intercultural understanding through intergroup dialogue between Japanese and Chinese university students. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 51(3).

  • Tajima, A. (2017). Using a “dialogic vaccine” to bridge opposing cultural viewpoints from the perspectives of Bakhtin’s theory on dialogue and estrangement. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. 51 (3).

  • Takahashi, N. & Yamamoto, T. (2016). Money as a cultural tool for East Asian children: Cultural developmental psychology of “pocket money”. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press [in Japanese].

  • Takahashi, N., Yamamoto, T., Takeo, K., Oh, S. A., Pian, C., & Sato, T. (2016). East Asian children and money as a cultural tool: dialectically understanding different cultures. Japanese Psychological Research, 58, 14–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder: Westview Press, Inc.

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Watanabe, T. (2017). The story-presenting method: a method for constructing the multiple viewpoint to understand different culture. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 51(3).

  • Wertsch, J. (1991). Voices of the Mind; A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Wertsch, J. (1998). Mind as action. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Yamamoto, T. (1991a). Establishment of the principle of “respect for occupancy” and its function in early children’s groups: On the problems of ontogeny of possession. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 122–132 [in Japanese].

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, T. (1991b). Possession consciousness in schoolchildren, and psychological weaning: Why do Japanese children go against Confucian principles? Nara Women’s University Annual Report of Pedagogy, 9, 93–113 [in Japanese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, T. (1992). Schoolchildren and their pocket money. Quarterly Hattatsu, 51, 68–76. Kyoto: Minerva Publishing [in Japanese].

  • Yamamoto, T. (1997). Development of possessive behavior and it’s cognitive structure in early childhood: A cross cultural study between Japanese and Chinese children. Doctoral thesis, Beijing Normal University [in Chinese]

  • Yamamoto, T. (2000). Children beginning to make play group: Autonomous play group and triad structure. In N. Okamoto & T. Asao (Eds.), Psychology of age (pp. 103–142). Kyoto: Minerva Publishing [in Japanese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, T. (2013). Ambiguity and substantiality as essential nature of culture: Redefinition of culture from the perspectives of “cultural psychology of difference”. Japanese Journal of Qualitative Psychology, 12, 44–65 [in Japanese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, T. (2015). What is culture? Where is it?: Psychology of conflict and symbiosis. Tokyo: Shinyosha Ltd. [in Japanese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, T., & Ito, T. (2005). Enjoying Asian movies with Asian people: new attempt for dialogical understanding of cultural others. Kyoto: Kitaohji [in Japanese].

  • Yamamoto, T. & Jiang, Y. (2011). Intercultural dialogue as a chain of gaps. In T. Yamamoto & K. Takagi (Eds.), Mind the gap: The psychology of dis-communication (pp 17–48). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press [in Japanese].

  • Yamamoto, T., & Takahashi, N. (2007). Money as a cultural tool mediating personal relationships: Child development of exchange and possession. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of sociocultural psychology (pp. 508–523). New York: Cambridge University press.

  • Yamamoto, T., & Takahashi, N. (in press). Possessions and money beyond market economy. In A. Rosa & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The  Cambridge Handbook of sociocultural psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Yamamoto, T., Takahashi, N., Sato, T., Takeo, K., Oh, S., & Pian, C. (2012). How can we study interactions mediated by money as a cultural tool. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 1056–1077). New York: Oxford University Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Toshiya Yamamoto.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yamamoto, T. Cultural Psychology of Differences and EMS; a New Theoretical Framework for Understanding and Reconstructing Culture. Integr. psych. behav. 51, 345–358 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9388-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9388-4

Keywords

Navigation