Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive Frames in Psychology: Demarcations and Ruptures

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

As there seems to be a recurrent feeling of crisis in psychology, its present state is analyzed in this article. The author believes that in addition to the traditional manifestations that have dogged psychology since it emerged as an independent science some new features of the crisis have emerged. Three fundamental “ruptures” are identified: the “horizontal” rupture between various schools and trends, the “vertical” rupture between natural science and humanitarian psychology, and the “diagonal” rupture between academic research and applied practice of psychology. These manifestations of the crisis of psychology have recently been compounded by the crisis of its rationalistic foundations. This situation is described in terms of the cognitive systems in psychology which include meta-theories, paradigms, sociodigms and metadigms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I emphasize the ad hoc nature of this “topography” which is here purely illustrative

  2. It is highly symptomatic that Thomas Kuhn described scientific schools as battle units of pre-paradigmatic science (Kuhn 1962)

  3. Perhaps these types of psychological study can be more usefully described as metaparadigms because the concept of “paradigm” clearly does not offer sufficient differentiating capacity to tell apart the main types of psychological research, and the complementary term “metaparadigm” is successfully used in sciences related to psychology, for example, sociology

  4. According to J. Pinel they include: 1) physiological psychology, 2) psychopharmacology, 3) neuropsychology, 4) psychophysiology and 5) comparative psychology (Pinel 1993)

  5. Calculations show that about 40% of psychological studies are experiment-based, 36% are based on polls, 16% on controlled observation, 4% on case studies and 3% on cross-cultural methods (Rosenzweig 1992)

  6. The author of this forecast predicts the possibility of telepathic capacity, which would make inroads on the mass media as a means of communication (Rosenzweig 1992)

  7. He also warns that as a result of this growing trend we run the risk of ending up with “psychology without science” (Polkinhorne 1994)

  8. Such irrationalization can manifest itself in a variety of ways. One sign is that the professions connected with the entertainment industry and generation of emotions (sports, show business, etc) are remunerated far more generously than the professions, for example, of a scientist which are connected with incremental increase of knowledge. The annual incomes of sports or show-biz stars who make no contribution to scientific-technical or social progress are multiples of the size of the Nobel Prize

  9. More can be identified

  10. A brilliant illustration is provided by the study of Kellog and Barron who have discovered that patients often prefer to be diagnosed with a more serious disease than to have no diagnosis because any explanation of their internal states has a tranquilizing effect (Kellog and Baron 1975)

  11. The author is grateful to Jaan Valsiner for prompting this metaphor and suggesting many other valuable ideas used in preparing this article

References

  • Agnew, N. M., & Pyke, S. W. (1969). The science game. And Introduction to Research in the Social Sciences. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.).(1988). The social psychology of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Campbell, D. T. (1988). Methodology and epistemology for social science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crombie, A. C. (1986). What is a history of science. History of European Ideas, 7(1), 21–31. doi:10.1016/0191-6599(86)90098-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1985). The methodological imperative in psychology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 15, 1–13. doi:10.1177/004839318501500101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eiduson, T., & Brown, H. (1962). Scientists: Their Psychological World. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. (1994). Toward a postmodern psychology. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmodernism (pp. 17–30). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granet, M. (1961). The Tao. In T. Parsons, E. Shils, K. D. Naegele, & J. R. Pitts (Eds.), Theories of society (vol. Vol II, (pp. 1098–1101)). New York: Free.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huffman, K., Vernoy, M., & Vernoy, J. (1994). Psychology in action. New York: Wiley & Sons, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (vol. Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, C. W. (1982). Theoretical perspectives in social psychology. Boston: Little, Brown and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, L. (1902). Life and letters of Thomas Henry Huxley. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hjelle, G. A., & Ziegler, D. J. (1992). Personality theories: Basic assumptions, research, and applications. New York: McGraw Hill, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellog, R., & Baron, R. S. (1975). Attribution theory, insomnia and the reverse placebo effect: reversal of storms and Nisbett findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 231–236. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M. J. (1976). Scientists as subjects: The psychological imperative. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1976). The Yin and Yang in social psychology: Seven Koan. In W. J. McGuire (Ed.), Social psychology in transition (pp. 33–50). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (2007). Psychology is about progress. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 41(1), 28–34. doi:10.1007/s12124-007-9008-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinel, J. P. J. (1993). Biopsychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, G. (1987). Of what the history of psychology is a history. British Journal for the History of Science, 20(65), 201–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie-Calder, L. (1976). The next billion years start now. In G. Leinwand (Ed.), The future (pp. 206–214). New York: Pocket Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, M. R. (1992). What is psychological science. In M. R. Rosenzeig (Ed.), International psychological science: Progress, problems, and prospects. Washington: American psychological assosiation.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Polkinhorne, D. E. (1994). Postmodern epistemology and practice. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmodernism (pp. 146–165). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagan, C. (1977). The Dragons of Eden. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sass, L. A. (1994). The epic of disbelief: The postmodernist turn in contemporary psychoanalysis. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmodernism (pp. 166–182). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N. W. (2001). Current systems in psychology: History, theory, research, and applications. Wadsworth: Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tele-interviews (2000). European Psychologist, 5(2).

  • The next twenty-five years of technology: opportunities and risks (1998). In 21-st century technologies: promises and perils of dynamic future (pp. 33–46). OECD.

  • Takasuna, M. (2007). Proliferation of western methodological thought in psychology in Japan: ways of objectification. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 41(1), 83–92. doi:10.1007/s12124-007-9006-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2007). Culture and science: strange history of the methodological thinking in psychology. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 41(1), 6–20. doi:10.1007/s12124-007-9004-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Vleist, R. (1982). Special psychological theory and empirical studies of practical problems. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Confronting social issues: Applications of social psychology (pp. 7–22). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1958). The protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. New York: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weimer, W. B. (1976). Psychology and the conceptual foundations of science. New Jersey: Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrey V. Yurevich.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Yurevich, A.V. Cognitive Frames in Psychology: Demarcations and Ruptures. Integr. psych. behav. 43, 89–103 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9082-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9082-7

Keywords

Navigation