Skip to main content
Log in

Ethics, evolution and culture

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Theory in Biosciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent work in the fields of evolutionary ethics and moral psychology appears to be converging on a single empirically- and evolutionary-based science of morality or ethics. To date, however, these fields have failed to provide an adequate conceptualisation of how culture affects the content and distribution of moral norms. This is particularly important for a large class of moral norms relating to rapidly changing technological or social environments, such as norms regarding the acceptability of genetically modified organisms. Here we suggest that a science of morality/ethics can benefit from adopting a cultural evolution or gene-culture coevolution approach, which treats culture as a second, separate evolutionary system that acts in parallel to biological/genetic evolution. This cultural evolution approach brings with it a set of established theoretical concepts (e.g. different cultural transmission mechanisms) and empirical methods (e.g. evolutionary game theory) that can significantly improve our understanding of human morality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Wilson et al. (2003) make the point that while descriptive facts about ethical beliefs, such as whether they are the product of natural selection, should not be the sole basis for normative theories of ethics, this does not mean that evolutionary origins are entirely irrelevant for normative theories. It seems to us that a normative theory that begins with an accurate understanding of why people hold the moral beliefs that they do would be superior to a normative theory that has no grounding in reality. Singer (2005) makes the additional point that ethicists commonly appeal to their own and others’ moral intuitions when constructing or criticising normative ethical theories. If these intuitions have an evolutionary basis, as is argued by many moral psychologists, then normative theories are already being influenced by evolutionary history, whether this is explicitly acknowledged or not.

  2. Models constructed by Boyd and Richerson (1985), Aoki et al. (2005) and Whitehead (2007) support our claim that environmental stability favours genetic control over behaviour, while environmental fluctuation favours learning. Strictly, these models show that rapid environmental change favours individual learning rather than cultural transmission, which is favoured at intermediate rates of environmental change. This contradicts our claim that rapidly changing moral norms are primarily culturally transmitted, and suggests instead that they should be acquired through individual learning. However, the efficacy of different moral norms are likely to be costly or difficult to assess through individual learning alone (how might a single member of the public, for example, determine the long-term health risks of GM food from non-GM food?). Other models (Boyd and Richerson 2005, chapters 1–2) suggest that cultural transmission is favoured when individual learning is costly or difficult, supporting our claim that moral norms will be primarily culturally transmitted rather than individually learned.

References

  • Aoki K, Wakano JY, Feldman MW (2005) The emergence of social learning in a temporally changing environment: a theoretical model. Curr Anthropol 46:334–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch SE (1951) Effects of group pressure on the modification and distortion of judgments. In: Guetzkow H (ed) Groups, leadership and men. Carnegie, Pittsburgh, pp 177–190

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran S (1998) Folk biology and the anthropology of science: cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behav Brain Sci 21:547–609

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aunger R (2000) The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos 28:1–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1984) The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayala FJ (2006) Biology to ethics: an evolutionist’s view of human nature. In: Boniolo G, De Anna G (eds) Evolutionary ethics and contemporary biology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 141–158

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) (1992) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley RA, Hahn MW, Shennan SJ (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:1443–1450

    Google Scholar 

  • Binmore K (1998) Game theory and the social contract II, just playing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond R, Smith P (1996) Culture and conformity: a meta-analysis of studies using asch’s line judgment task. Psychol Bull 119:111–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Boniolo G, De Anna G (2006) Evolutionary ethics and contemporary biology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proc Natl Acad Sci 100:3531–3535

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell DT (1974) Evolutionary epistemology. In: Schilpp PA (ed) The philosophy of Karl Popper. Open Court, La Salle, pp 413–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ (2004) Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu Rev Psychol 55:591–621

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton P, Schloss J (2004) Evolution and ethics. Eerdmans, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Croft W (2000) Explaining language change: an evolutionary approach. Longman, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Cushman F, Young L, Hauser MD (2006) The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: testing three principles of harm. Psychol Sci 17:1082–1089

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danielson P (1992) Artificial morality: virtuous robots for virtual games. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Danielson P (1993) Personal responsibility [for global warming]. In: Hurka T, Coward H (eds) The ethics of atmospheric change. Wilfred Laurier Press, Waterloo, pp 81–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Danielson P (2002) Competition among cooperators: altruism and reciprocity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99:7237–7242

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Danielson P (2007) The place of ethics in a unified behavioral science. Behav Brain Sci 30:23–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1859/1968) The origin of species. Penguin, London

  • Darwin C (1871/2003) The descent of man. Gibson Square, London

  • de Waal F (1996) Good natured: the origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine D, Clayton L, Dunford B, Seying R, Pryce J (2001) Jury decision making: 45 years of empirical research on deliberating groups. Psychol Public Policy Law 7:622–727

    Google Scholar 

  • Doris J, Stich S (2006) Moral psychology: empirical approaches. In: Zalta EN (ed) Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Durham WH (1991) Coevolution: genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich PR (2001) Intervening in evolution: ethics and actions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5477–5480

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elena SF, Lenski RE (2003) Evolution experiments with microorganisms: the dynamics and genetic bases of adaptation. Nat Rev Genet 4:457–469

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman MW, Laland KN (1996) Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends Ecol Evol 11:453–457

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessler D, Navarrete C (2003) Meat is good to taboo: dietary proscriptions as a product of the interaction of psychological mechanisms and social processes. J Cogn Cul 3:1–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell G, Allum N, Bauer M, Jackson J, Howard S, Lindsey N (2003) Climate change for biotechnology? UK public opinion 1991–2002. AgBioForum 6:55–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell G, Bauer M, Durant J, Allum N (1999) Worlds apart? The reception of genetically modified foods in Europe and the US. Science 285:384–387

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Game theory evolving. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2007) A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences. Behav Brain Sci 30:1–61

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould JL (1986) The biology of learning. Annu Rev Psychol 37:163–192

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of san marco and the panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc B 205:581–598

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gray RD, Atkinson QD (2003) Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of indo-European origin. Nature 426:435–439

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greene J (2003) From neural is to moral ought: what are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nat Rev Neurosci 4:846–849

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greene J, Haidt J (2002) How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends Cogn Sci 6:517–523

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene J, Sommerville R, Nystrom L, Darley J, Cohen J (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293:2105–2108

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven M, Zanolini A, Schniter E (2008) Culture sometimes matters: intra-cultural variation in pro-social behavior among Tsimane Amerindians. J Econ Behav Organ

  • Haidt J (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol Rev 108:814–834

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt J (2007) The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science 316:998–1002

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour I and II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hartl DL, Clark AG (1997) Principles of population genetics. Sinauer, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey PH, Pagel MD (1991) The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (2006a) Moral ingredients: how we evolved the capacity to do the right thing. In: Levinson SC, Jaisson P (eds) Evolution and culture. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 219–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (2006b) Moral minds: how nature designed a universal sense right and wrong. Harper Collins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Young L, Cushman FA (2008) Reviving rawls’ linguistic analogy. In: Sinnott-Armstrong W (eds) The biology and psychology of morality. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 107–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol Hum Behav 19:215–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R, Alvard M, Barr A, Ensminger J, Henrich NS, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe FW, Patton JQ, Tracer D (2005) “Economic Man” in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci 28:795–855

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isenberg D (1986) Group polarization: a critical review and meta-analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 50:1141–1151

    Google Scholar 

  • James W (1880) Great men, great thoughts, and the environment. Atl Mon 46:441–459

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenski RE, Travisano M (1994) Dynamics of adaptation and diversification: a 10,000-generation experiment with bacterial-populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci 91:6808–6814

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lipo CP, O’Brien MJ, Collard M, Shennan S (eds) (2006) Mapping Our Ancestors: Phylogenetic Approaches in Anthropology and Prehistory. Aldine, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Luskin RC, Fishkin JS, Jowell R (2002) Considered opinions: deliberative polling in Britain. Br J Polit Sci 32:455–487

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCoun RJ (1989) Experimental research on jury decision-making. Science 244:1046

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mace R, Holden CJ (2005) A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 20:116–121

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr E, Provine W (eds) (1980) The evolutionary synthesis. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath R, Lubell M, Richerson PJ, Waring TM, Baum W, Edsten E, Efferson C, Paciotti B (2005) Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evol Hum Behav 26:483–508

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2007) Using the methods of social psychology to study cultural evolution. J Soc Evol Cult Psychol 1:35–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2008) A Darwinian theory of cultural evolution can promote an evolutionary synthesis for the social sciences. Biol Theory 2:263–275

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Danielson P (2007) Parallel ethical worlds: an experimental design for applied ethics. Electronic working papers Series. W. Maurice Young centre for applied ethics, University of British Columbia at http://www.gels.ethics.ubc.ca

  • Mesoudi A, Laland KN (2007) Culturally transmitted paternity beliefs and the evolution of human mating behaviour. Proc R Soc B 274:1273–1278

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, O’Brien MJ (2008) The cultural transmission of great basin projectile point technology I: an experimental simulation. Am Antiq 73:3–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Dunbar RIM (2006a) A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. Br J Psychol 97:405–423

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2004) Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the Origin of Species. Evolution 58:1–11

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2006b) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav Brain Sci 29:329–383

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milgram S (1974) Obedience to authority. Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mufwene SS (2001) The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller M (1870) The science of language. Nature 1:256–259

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols S (2002) On the genealogy of norms: a case for the role of emotion in cultural evolution. Philos Sci 69:234–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Nitecki MH, Nitecki DV (1993) Evolutionary ethics. SUNY Press, Albany

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan A, Atran S (2004) Cognitive and emotional processes in the cultural transmission of natural and nonnatural beliefs. In: Schaller M, Crandall C (eds) The psychological foundations of culture. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 149–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan A, Atran S, Faulkner J, Schaller M (2006). Memory and mystery: the cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cogn Sci 30:531–553

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien MJ, Lyman RL (2003) Cladistics and archaeology. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City

    Google Scholar 

  • Oudeyer P, Kaplan F (2007) Language evolution as a Darwinian process: computational studies. Cogn Process 8:21–35

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1997) How the mind works. W. W. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitt-Rivers AL (1875) On the evolution of culture. J Anthropol Inst 4:293–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin H (ed) (1982) Learning, development, and culture: essays in evolutionary epistemology. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper KR (1979) Objective knowledge: an evolutionary approach. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Pusey A, Wolf M (1996) Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends Ecol Evol 11:201–206

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawls J (1971) A theory of justice. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards RJ (1986) A defense of evolutionary ethics. Biol Philos 1:265–293

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R, Henrich J (2003) Cultural evolution of human cooperation. In: Hammerstein P (eds) The genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 357–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruse M, Wilson E (1986) Moral philosophy as applied science. Philosophy 61:173–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli CA, Weary DM (2007) Multiple uses of pigs: an interactive survey to assess people’s attitudes towards animal use and genetic modification. In: Moving mountains, 46th annual symposium of The association for laboratory animal science, Calgary, Alberta, 2–5 June 2007, p 30

  • Singer P (1982) Ethics and sociobiology. Philos Public Aff 11:40–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer P (2005) Ethics and intuitions. J Ethics 9:331–352

    Google Scholar 

  • Skyrms B (1996) Evolution of the social contract. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith EA, Winterhalder B (eds) (1992) Evolutionary ecology and human behavior. Aldine de Gruyter, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sober E, Wilson DS (1998) Unto others: the evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Hirschfeld LA (2004) The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends Cogn Sci 8:40–46

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sripada CS, Stich S (2006) A framework for the psychology of norms. In: Carruthers P, Laurence S, Stich S (eds) The innate mind: culture and cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 280–301

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein C (2002) The law of group polarization. J Polit Philos 10:175–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol 20:410–433

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers R (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Wald KD, Calhoun-Brown A (2007) Religion and politics in the United States. Rowman and Littlefield, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead H (2007) Learning, climate and the evolution of cultural capacity. J Theor Biol 245:341–350

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson D, Dietrich E, Clark A (2003) On the inappropriate use of the naturalistic fallacy in evolutionary psychology. Biol Philos 18:669–681

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder B, Smith EA (2000) Analyzing adaptive strategies: human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evol Anthropol 9:51–72

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Roger Stanev and two anonymous reviewers for useful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. This work was partly funded by Genome Canada through the offices of Genome British Columbia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex Mesoudi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mesoudi, A., Danielson, P. Ethics, evolution and culture. Theory Biosci. 127, 229–240 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-008-0027-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-008-0027-y

Keywords

Navigation