Skip to main content
Log in

Awareness of Memory Ability and Change: (In)Accuracy of Memory Self-Assessments in Relation to Performance

  • Published:
Journal of Population Ageing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Little is known about subjective assessments of memory abilities and decline among middle-aged adults or their association with objective memory performance in the general population. In this study we examined self-ratings of memory ability and change in relation to episodic memory performance in two national samples of middle-aged and older adults from the Midlife in the United States study (MIDUS II in 2005–06) and the Health and Retirement Study (HRS; every 2 years from 2002 to 2012). MIDUS (Study 1) participants (N =3581) rated their memory compared to others their age and to themselves 5 years ago; HRS (Study 2) participants (N = 14,821) rated their current memory and their memory compared to 2 years ago, with up to six occasions of longitudinal data over 10 years. In both studies, episodic memory performance was the total number of words recalled in immediate and delayed conditions. When controlling for demographic and health correlates, self-ratings of memory abilities, but not subjective change, were related to performance. We examined accuracy by comparing subjective and objective memory ability and change. More than one third of the participants across the studies had self-assessments that were inaccurate relative to their actual level of performance and change, and accuracy differed as a function of demographic and health factors. Further understanding of self-awareness of memory abilities and change beginning in midlife may be useful for identifying early warning signs of decline, with implications regarding policies and practice for early detection and treatment of cognitive impairment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because our total sample also included siblings of the main RDD respondents and a subpopulation of twins, all models were also estimated using the cluster option in STATA (StataCorp 2009). This option takes dependencies into account using robust standard errors by clustering at the family level. The results of these analyses revealed similar patterns. As sample weights were available for only the random-digit dial RDD sample of MIDUS participants, we did not apply them in the analyses.

  2. Participants 90 and older at baseline were excluded in order to examine longitudinal change in cognitive data and because of the lack of follow-up data available from those aged 90 and older following the 2002 interview.

  3. Analyses were conducted using sample weights to account for oversampling of Black and Hispanic individuals and Florida residents, and to reflect the United States census population in 2002 at baseline for the current study (Heeringa and Connor 1995). Results were similar, with a few exceptions. In Model 1, with memory performance as an outcome, the interaction between SRH and time was no longer significant. In Model 3, with perceived memory change as an outcome, the effect of race was no longer significant and the effect of memory performance became significant. Also, due to the nature of the study design, some participants from the same households are included in the HRS. Thus, multilevel models were used to examine three-levels (level 1: within-person, level 2: within-household, and level 3: between-households) to account for the clustering of data and sample dependencies. These findings revealed no differences from the two level model examining within-person and between-person change over time and therefore are not reported.

  4. Memory change was examined in all other years, 2004 to 2006, 2006 to 2008, 2008 to 2010 and 2010 to 2012 in relation to self-ratings of perceived change. There were similar rates of decline (2004–6: 13.3 %, 2006–8: 14.3 %; 2008–10: 17.5 %; 2010–12, 15.1 %) and there were similar rates of accuracy and inaccuracy; thus for parsimony only results from the baseline year are reported in Fig. 6.

References

  • Aarts, S., van den Akker, M., Hajema, K. J., van Ingen, A. M., Metsemakers, J. F., Verhey, F. R., & van Boxtel, M. P. (2011). Multimorbidity and its relation to subjective memory complaints in a large general population of older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 23(4), 616–624. doi:10.1017/S1041610210002024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Agrigoroaei, S., & Lachman, M. E. (2011). Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66, 130–140. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, M. S., Jones, K., Savage, C. R., Berkman, L., Seeman, T., Blazer, D., & Rowe, J. W. (1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychology and Aging, 10, 578–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, M. S., Moss, M. B., Blacker, D., Tanzi, R., & McArdle, J. J. (2007). Longitudinal change in cognitive performance among individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 21(2), 158–169. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., & Petersen, R. C. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(3), 270–279. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alzheimer’s Association. (2014). 2014 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(2), e47–e92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amariglio, R. E., Becker, J. A., Carmasin, J., Wadsworth, L. P., Lorius, N., Sullivan, C., & Rentz, D. M. (2012). Subjective cognitive complaints and amyloid burden in cognitively normal older individuals. Neuropsychologia, 50(12), 2880–2886. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bäckman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A.-K., Laukka, E. J., & Small, B. J. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 19(4), 520–531. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37(2), 122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassett, S. S., & Folstein, M. F. (1993). Memory complaint, memory performance, and psychiatric diagnosis: a community study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 6(2), 105–111. doi:10.1177/089198879300600207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2011). Are memory self-efficacy and memory performance related? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 211–241. doi:10.1037/a0022106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1997). Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiology of Aging, 18(4), 351–357. doi:10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00056-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caracciolo, B., Gatz, M., Xu, W., Pedersen, N. L., & Fratiglioni, L. (2012). Differential distribution of subjective and objective cognitive impairment in the population: a nation-wide twin-study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 29(2), 393–403. doi:10.3233/JAD-2011-111904.

    Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2013). Self-reported increased confusion or memory loss and associated functional difficulties among adults aged >/= 60 years. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 62, 347–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coley, N., Ousset, P., Andrieu, S., Mathiex-Fortunet, H., & Vellas, B. (2008). Memory complaints to the general practitioner: data from the GuidAge study. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 12(1), S66–S72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comijs, H., Deeg, D., Dik, M., Twisk, J., & Jonker, C. (2002). Memory complaints; the association with psycho-affective and health problems and the role of personality characteristics: a 6-year follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72(2), 157–165. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00453-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corner, L., & Bond, J. (2004). Being at risk of dementia: fears and anxieties of older adults. Journal of Aging Studies, 18(2), 143–155. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2004.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane, M., Bogner, H., Brown, G., & Gallo, J. (2007). The link between depressive symptoms, negative cognitive bias and memory complaints in older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 11(6), 708–715. doi:10.1080/13607860701368497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crimmins, E. M., Kim, J. K., Langa, K. M., & Weir, D. R. (2011). Assessment of cognition using surveys and neuropsychological assessment: the Health and Retirement Study and the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66(suppl 1), i162–i171. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crumley, J. J., Stetler, C. A., & Horhota, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between subjective and objective memory performance in older adults: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 29(2), 250–263. doi:10.1037/a0035908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daviglus, M. L., Bell, C. C., Berrettini, W., Bowen, P. E., Connolly, E. S., Cox, N. J., Dunbar-Jacob, J. M., Granieri, E. C., Hunt, G., McGarry, K., Patel, D., Potosky, A. L., Sanders-Bush, E., Silberberg, D., & Trevisan, M. (2010). NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement: preventing Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline. NIH Consensus and State-of-the-Science Statements, 27(4), 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. A. (2000). The concept of metamemory: Cognitive, developmental, and clinical issues. In G. E. Berrios & J. R. Hodges (Eds.), Memory disorders in psychiatric practice (pp. 47–57). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., DeKosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., & Scheltens, P. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology, 6(8), 734–746. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dux, M. C., Woodard, J. L., Calamari, J. E., Messina, M., Arora, S., Chik, H., & Amore, M. (2008). The moderating role of negative affect on objective verbal memory performance and subjective memory complaints in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(2), 327–336. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • González, H. M., Bowen, M. E., & Fisher, G. G. (2008). Memory decline and depressive symptoms in a nationally representative sample of older adults: the Health and Retirement Study (1998–2004). Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25(3), 266–271. doi:10.1159/000115976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grut, M., Jorm, A. F., Fratiglioni, L., Forsell, Y., Viitanen, M., & Winblad, B. (1993). Memory complaints of elderly people in a population survey: variation according to dementia stage and depression. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 41(12), 1295–1300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guenther, C. L., & Alicke, M. D. (2010). Deconstructing the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 755–770. doi:10.1037/a0020959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, E. A., & Lachman, M. E. (2014). Everyday experiences of memory problems and control: the adaptive role of selective optimization with compensation in the context of memory decline. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. doi:10.1080/13825585.2014.888391. Advance online publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. B., Derby, C., LeValley, A., Katz, M. J., Verghese, J., & Lipton, R. B. (2007). Education delays accelerated decline on a memory test in persons who develop dementia. Neurology, 69(17), 1657–1664. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000278163.82636.30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heeringa, S. G., & Connor, J. H. (1995). Technical description of the Health and Retirement Survey sample design. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 52–56. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00547.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Metacognition in later adulthood: spared monitoring can benefit older adults’ self-regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 167–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, A. R., & Wallace, R. B. (1997). Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD study. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 52B(Special Issue), 37–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulur, G., Hertzog, C., Pearman, A., Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2014). Longitudinal associations of subjective memory with memory performance and depressive symptoms: between-person and within-person perspectives. Psychology and Aging. doi:10.1037/a0037619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chetelat, G., Dubois, B., Dufouil, C., Ellis, K. A., van der Flier, W. M., Glodzik, L., van Harten, A. C., de Leon, M. J., McHugh, P., Mielke, M. M., Molinuevo, J. L., Mosconi, L., Osorio, R. S., Perrotin, A., Petersen, R. C., Rabin, L. A., Rami, L., Reisberg, B., Rentz, D. M., Sachdev, P. S., de la Sayette, V., Saykin, A. J., Scheltens, P., Shulman, M. B., Slavin, M. J., Sperling, R. A., Stewart, R., Uspenskaya, O., Vellas, B., Visser, P. J., Wagner, M., & Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonker, C., Smits, C. H. M., & Deeg, D. J. H. (1997). Affect-related metamemory and memory performance in a population-based sample of older adults. Educational Gerontology, 23, 115–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A., Christensen, H., Korten, A., Henderson, A., Jacomb, P., & Mackinnon, A. (1997). Do cognitive complaints either predict future cognitive decline or reflect past cognitive decline? A longitudinal study of an elderly community sample. Psychological Medicine, 27(1), 91–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juster, F. T., & Suzman, R. (1995). An overview of the health and retirement study. Journal of Human Resources, 30, S7–S56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karlamangla, A. S., Miller-Martinez, D., Aneshensel, C. S., Seeman, T. E., Wight, R. G., & Chodosh, J. (2009). Trajectories of cognitive function in late life in the United States: demographic and socioeconomic predictors. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170(3), 331–342. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohout, F. J., Berkman, L. F., Evans, D. A., & Cornoni-Huntley, J. (1993). Two shorter forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. Journal of Aging and Health, 5(2), 179–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kryscio, R. J., Abner, E. L., Cooper, G. E., Fardo, D. W., Jicha, G. A., Nelson, P. T., & Schmitt, F. A. (2014). Self-reported memory complaints: implications from a longitudinal cohort with autopsies. Neurology, 83(15), 1359–1365. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000000856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E. (2004). Development in midlife. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 305–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., & Tun, P. A. (2008). Cognitive testing in large-scale surveys: Assessment by telephone. In S. Hofer & D. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook on cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 506–523). Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1997). The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) personality scales: Scale construction and scoring. Waltham, MA: Brandeis University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., Agrigoroaei, S., Tun, P. A., & Weaver, S. L. (2014). Monitoring cognitive functioning: psychometric properties of the brief test of adult cognition by telephone. Assessment, 21, 404–417. doi:10.1177/1073191113508807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langa, K. M., Plassman, B. L., Wallace, R. B., Herzog, A. R., Heeringa, S. G., Ofstedal, M. B., & Hurd, M. D. (2005). The aging, demographics, and memory study: study design and methods. Neuroepidemiology, 25(4), 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langa, K. M., Llewellyn, D. J., Lang, I. A., Weir, D. R., Wallace, R. B., Kabeto, M. U., & Huppert, F. A. (2009). Cognitive health among older adults in the United States and in England. BMC Geriatrics, 9(1), 23. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-9-23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P.-L. (2014). The relationship between memory complaints, activity and perceived health status. Health and Disability, 55, 136–141. doi:10.1111/sjop.12107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P.-L., Hsiao, C.-H., & Wang, C.-L. (2013). Physical activity and memory complaints in middle-age Americans: results from the MIDUS study. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 28(6), 600–605. doi:10.1177/1533317513494744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J., Fisher, G. G., & Kadlec, K. M. (2007). Latent variable analyses of age trends of cognition in the Health and Retirement Study, 1992–2004. Psychology and Aging, 22(3), 525–545. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merema, M. R., Speelman, C. P., Foster, J. K., & Kaczmarek, E. A. (2012). Neuroticism (not depressive symptoms) predicts memory complaints in some community-dwelling older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(8), 729–736. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e31826d6973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mewton, L., Sachdev, P., Anderson, T., Sunderland, M., & Andrews, G. (2013). Demographic, clinical, and lifestyle correlates of subjective memory complaints in the Australian population. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2013.04.004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, A. J. (2008). The clinical significance of subjective memory complaints in the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and dementia: a meta-analysis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(11), 1191–1202. doi:10.1002/gps.2053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, A. J., Beaumont, H., Ferguson, D., Yadegarfar, M., & Stubbs, B. (2014). Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. doi:10.1111/acps.12336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mol, M., Carpay, M., Ramakers, I., Rozendaal, N., Verhey, F., & Jolles, J. (2007). The effect of perceived forgetfulness on quality of life in older adults; a qualitative review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(5), 393–400. doi:10.1002/gps.1686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mol, M., van Boxtel, M. P., Willems, D., Verhey, F. R., & Jolles, J. (2009). Subjective forgetfulness is associated with lower quality of life in middle-aged and young-old individuals: a 9-year follow-up in older participants from the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging & Mental Health, 13(5), 699–705. doi:10.1080/13607860902845541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695–699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ofstedal, M. B., Zimmer, Z. S., & Lin, H.-S. (1999). A comparison of correlates of cognitive functioning in older persons in Taiwan and the United States. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54(5), S291–S301. doi:10.1093/geronb/54B.5.S291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ofstedal, M. B., Fisher, G. G., & Hertzog, A. R. (2005). Documentation of cognitive functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183–194. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassman, B. L., Langa, K. M., Fisher, G. G., Heeringa, S. G., Weir, D. R., Ofstedal, M. B., & Rodgers, W. L. (2008). Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Annals of Internal Medicine, 148(6), 427–434. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-148-6-200803180-00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassman, B. L., Langa, K. M., McCammon, R. J., Fisher, G. G., Potter, G. G., Burke, J. R., & Unverzagt, F. W. (2011). Incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment, not dementia in the United States. Annals of Neurology, 70(3), 418–426. doi:10.1002/ana.22362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podewils, L. J., McLay, R. N., Rebok, G. W., & Lyketsos, C. G. (2003). Relationship of self-perceptions of memory and worry to objective measures of memory and cognition in the general population. Psychosomatics, 44(6), 461–470. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.44.6.461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radler, B. T., & Ryff, C. D. (2010). Who participates? Accounting for longitudinal retention in the MIDUS national study of health and well-being. Journal of Aging and Health, 22, 307–331. doi:10.1177/0898264309358617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The Ces-D scale. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, L. M., & Maclullich, A. M. (2006). Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22(5–6), 471–485. doi:10.1159/000096295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaries De France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J., Clare, L., & Woods, R. (2009). Subjective memory complaints and awareness of memory functioning in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(2), 95–109. doi:10.1159/000234911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, M., Tym, E., Mountjoy, C., Huppert, F. A., Hendrie, H., Verma, S., & Goddard, R. (1986). CAMDEX. A standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 149(6), 698–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rouch, I., Anterion, C. T., Dauphinot, V., Kerleroux, J., Roche, F., Barthelemy, J. C., & Laurent, B. (2008). Cognitive complaints, neuropsychological performance and affective disorders in elderly community residents. Disability and Rehabilitation, 30(23), 1794–1802. doi:10.1080/09638280701667825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. (2009). Major issues in cognitive aging. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M., Taylor, M. A., & Haitiner, A. (1994). Personality and self-report of cognitive functioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9(4), 353–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh-Manoux, A., Kivimaki, M., Glymour, M. M., Elbaz, A., Berr, C., Ebmeier, K. P., & Dugravot, A. (2012). Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal, 344, d7622. doi:10.1136/Bmj.D7622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, M. J., Brodaty, H., Kochan, N. A., Crawford, J. D., Trollor, J. N., Draper, B., & Sachdev, P. S. (2010). Prevalence and predictors of “subjective cognitive complaints” in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(8), 701–710. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181df49fb.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, B. J., Dixon, R. A., & McArdle, J. J. (2011). Tracking cognition–health changes from 55 to 95 years of age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66(suppl 1), i153–i161. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snitz, B. E., Morrow, L. A., Rodriguez, E. G., Huber, K. A., & Saxton, J. A. (2008). Subjective memory complaints and concurrent memory performance in older patients of primary care providers. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(6), 1004–1013. doi:10.1017/S1355617708081332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • StataCorp. (2009). Stata statistical software (Version 11). College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steffick, D. E. (2000). Documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor: HRS Health Working Group.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tun, P. A., & Lachman, M. E. (2008). Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1421–1429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, C. L., Schultz, S., Arndt, S., Wallace, R. B., & Herzog, R. (2000). Memory complaint in a community sample aged 70 and older. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48(11), 1435–1441.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Harten, A. C., Smits, L. L., Teunissen, C. E., Visser, P. J., Koene, T., Blankenstein, M. A., & van der Flier, W. M. (2013). Preclinical AD predicts decline in memory and executive functions in subjective complaints. Neurology, 81(16), 1409–1416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Oijen, M., de Jong, F. J., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P. J., & Breteler, M. M. B. (2007). Subjective memory complaints, education, and risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 3(2), 92–97. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldorff, F. B., Rishoj, S., & Waldemar, G. (2008). If you don’t ask (about memory), they probably won’t tell. Journal of Family Practice, 57(1), 41–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, P. S., Berglund, P., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Recent care of common mental disorders in the United States. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 15(5), 284–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ware, J. E., Jr., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, P. J., & Moody, H. R. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment: a ‘hardening of the categories’? Dementia, 5(1), 11–25. doi:10.1177/1471301206059752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zandi, T. (2004). Relationship between subjective memory complaints, objective memory performance, and depression among older adults. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 19(6), 353–360. doi:10.1177/153331750401900610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelinski, E. M., Burnight, K. P., & Lane, C. J. (2001). The relationship between subjective and objective memory in the oldest old: comparisons of findings from a representative and a convenience sample. Journal of Aging and Health, 13, 248–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from the National Institute on Aging (Grants PO1 AG20166 and RO1 AG17920).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Margie E. Lachman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rickenbach, E.H., Agrigoroaei, S. & Lachman, M.E. Awareness of Memory Ability and Change: (In)Accuracy of Memory Self-Assessments in Relation to Performance. Population Ageing 8, 71–99 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12062-014-9108-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12062-014-9108-5

Keywords

Navigation