Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Examining the relationship between home literacy environment and neural correlates of phonological processing in beginning readers with and without a familial risk for dyslexia: an fMRI study

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Developmental dyslexia is a language-based learning disability characterized by persistent difficulty in learning to read. While an understanding of genetic contributions is emerging, the ways the environment affects brain functioning in children with developmental dyslexia are poorly understood. A relationship between the home literacy environment (HLE) and neural correlates of reading has been identified in typically developing children, yet it remains unclear whether similar effects are observable in children with a genetic predisposition for dyslexia. Understanding environmental contributions is important given that we do not understand why some genetically at-risk children do not develop dyslexia. Here, we investigate for the first time the relationship between HLE and the neural correlates of phonological processing in beginning readers with (FHD+, n = 29) and without (FHD−, n = 21) a family history of developmental dyslexia. We further controlled for socioeconomic status to isolate the neurobiological mechanism by which HLE affects reading development. Group differences revealed stronger correlation of HLE with brain activation in the left inferior/middle frontal and right fusiform gyri in FHD− compared to FHD+ children, suggesting greater impact of HLE on manipulation of phonological codes and recruitment of orthographic representations in typically developing children. In contrast, activation in the right precentral gyrus showed a significantly stronger correlation with HLE in FHD+ compared to FHD− children, suggesting emerging compensatory networks in genetically at-risk children. Overall, our results suggest that genetic predisposition for dyslexia alters contributions of HLE to early reading skills before formal reading instruction, which has important implications for educational practice and intervention models.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: learning and thinking about print. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barquero, L. A., Davis, N., & Cutting, L. E. (2014). Neuroimaging of reading intervention: a systematic review and activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis. PloS One, 9(1), e83668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (2007). Covariance structure models for maximal reliability of unit-weighted composites. In S. Lee (Ed.), Handbook of computing and statistics with applications: Vol. 1. Handbook of latent variable and related models (pp. 1–19). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Bebko, G., Burman, D. D., & Bitan, T. (2007). Children with reading disorder show modality independent brain abnormalities during semantic tasks. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 775–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brambati, S. M., Termine, C., Ruffino, M., Danna, M., Lanzi, G., Stella, G., et al. (2006). Neuropsychological deficits and neural dysfunction in familial dyslexia. Brain Research, 1113(1), 174–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brito, N. H., & Noble, K. G. (2014). Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8.

  • Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M., Coalson, R. S., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2005). Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cerebral Cortex, 15(3), 275–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, S. R., Hecht, S. A., & Lonigan, C. J. (2002). Relations of the home literacy environment (HLE) to the development of reading‐related abilities: a one-year longitudinal study. Reading Research Quarterly, 37(4), 408–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65(1), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T. L., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2006). Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(10), 1041–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christopher, M. E., Hulslander, J., Byrne, B., Samuelsson, S., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., et al. (2015). Genetic and environmental etiologies of the longitudinal relations between prereading skills and reading. Child Development, 86(2), 342–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L., Martinaud, O., Lemer, C., Lehericy, S., Samson, Y., Obadia, M., et al. (2003). Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. Cerebral Cortex, 13(12), 1313–1333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1999). The problem of units and the circumstance for POMP. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 34(3), 315–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darki, F., Peyrard-Janvid, M., Matsson, H., Kere, J., & Klingberg, T. (2012). Three dyslexia susceptibility genes, DYX1C1, DCDC2, and KIAA0319, affect temporo-parietal white matter structure. Biological Psychiatry, 72(8), 671–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, P. F., & Leseman, P. P. (2001). Lasting effects of home literacy on reading achievement in school. Journal of School Psychology, 39(5), 389–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries, J. C., & Alarcón, M. (1996). Genetics of specific reading disability. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 2(1), 39–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., et al. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330(6009), 1359–1364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desroches, A. S., Cone, N. E., Bolger, D. J., Bitan, T., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2010). Children with reading difficulties show differences in brain regions associated with orthographic processing during spoken language processing. Brain Research, 1356, 73–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dilnot, J., Hamilton, L., Maughan, B., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). Child and environmental risk factors predicting readiness for learning in children at high risk of dyslexia. Development and Psychopathology, FirstView, 1–10.

  • Drakesmith, M., El-Deredy, W., & Welbourne, S. (2015). Differential phonological and semantic modulation of neurophysiological responses to visual word recognition. Neuropsychobiology, 72(1), 46–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: phases and findings.

  • Eilam-Stock, T., Xu, P., Cao, M., Gu, X., Van Dam, N. T., Anagnostou, E., et al. (2014). Abnormal autonomic and associated brain activities during rest in autism spectrum disorder. Brain, 137(1), 153–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, J. C., Barron, R. W., & Brunello, M. (2000). Direct and mediated influences of home literacy and literacy interest on prereaders’ oral vocabulary and early written language skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D., & Glover, G. H. (2007a). Assessing the influence of scanner background noise on auditory processing. I. An fMRI study comparing three experimental designs with varying degrees of scanner noise. Human Brain Mapping, 28(8), 703–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D., & Glover, G. H. (2007b). Assessing the influence of scanner background noise on auditory processing. II. An fMRI study comparing auditory processing in the absence and presence of recorded scanner noise using a sparse design. Human Brain Mapping, 28(8), 721–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D., & Glover, G. H. (2008). Resting in peace or noise: scanner background noise suppresses default‐mode network. Human Brain Mapping, 29(7), 858–867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., LoTurco, J., Ramus, F., Fitch, R. H., & Rosen, G. D. (2006). From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1213–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., & Rosen, G. D. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomahes. Reading, 6, 1.8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geldhof, G. J., Preacher, K. J., & Zyphur, M. J. (2014). Reliability estimation in a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis framework. Psychological Methods, 19, 72–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. L. (2004). Genetic bases of developmental dyslexia: a capsule review of heritability estimates.

  • Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 65–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, N., Hanson, J., Pollak, S., & Wolfe, B. (2013). Socioeconomic differences and academic achievement: insights from the developing brain. Washington: In APPAM Annual Fall Research Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, D. A., Haggard, M. P., Akeroyd, M. A., Palmer, A. R., Summerfield, A. Q., Elliott, M. R., et al. (1999). Sparse” temporal sampling in auditory fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 7(3), 213–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, L. (2013). The role of the home literacy environment in the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia.

  • Hanson, J. L., Hair, N., Shen, D. G., Shi, F., Gilmore, J. H., Wolfe, B. L., et al. (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth.

  • Hoeft, F., McCandliss, B. D., Black, J. M., Gantman, A., Zakerani, N., Hulme, C., et al. (2011). Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(1), 361–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A., Juel, C., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J. L., et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(10), 4234–4239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E., & Tardif, T. (1995). Socioeconomic status and parenting.

  • Holland, S. K., Plante, E., Byars, A. W., Strawsburg, R. H., Schmithorst, V. J., & Ball, W. S. (2001). Normal fMRI brain activation patterns in children performing a verb generation task. NeuroImage, 14(4), 837–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus, T., Vannest, J. J., & Holland, S. K. (2013). Overlapping neural circuitry for narrative comprehension and proficient reading in children and adolescents. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2651–2662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B., & Hickok, G. (2001). Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(8), 1749–1752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutton, J. S., Horowitz-Kraus, T., Mendelsohn, A. L., DeWitt, T., & Holland, S. K. (2015). Home reading environment and brain activation in preschool children listening to stories. Pediatrics, 136(3), 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ihaka, R., & Gentleman, R. (1996). R: a language for data analysis and graphics. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 5(3), 299–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J., Hawco, C., Kobayashi, E., Boor, R., LeVan, P., Stephani, U., et al. (2008). Variability of the hemodynamic response as a function of age and frequency of epileptic discharge in children with epilepsy. NeuroImage, 40(2), 601–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jednoróg, K., Altarelli, I., Monzalvo, K., Fluss, J., Dubois, J., Billard, C., et al. (2012). The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PloS One, 7(8), e42486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kere, J. (2014). The molecular genetics and neurobiology of developmental dyslexia as model of a complex phenotype. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications, 452(2), 236–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, G. M., Duda, J. T., Avants, B. B., Wu, J., & Farah, M. J. (2013). Associations between children's socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental Science, 16(5), 641–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A., Gong, Z., Hessels, S., Evans, M. A., & Jared, D. (2006). Understanding print: early reading development and the contributions of home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(1), 63–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D., & Cunningham, W. A. (2009). Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 423–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J. (1994). Reading to preschoolers exposed: is the emperor really naked? Developmental Review, 14(3), 303–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36(5), 596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Olofsson, Å., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21(1), 159–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, T., & Hayasaka, S. (2003). Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 12(5), 419–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Home literacy environment and the beginning of reading and spelling. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(1), 40–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., et al. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 773–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., & Sowell, E. R. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental Science, 15(4), 516–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Brain–behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science, 9(6), 642–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, B. A., Wolf, M., & Lovett, M. W. (2012). A taxometric investigation of developmental dyslexia subtypes. Dyslexia, 18(1), 16–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obleser, J., Boecker, H., Drzezga, A., Haslinger, B., Hennenlotter, A., Roettinger, M., et al. (2006). Vowel sound extraction in anterior superior temporal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 27(7), 562–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozernov-Palchik, O., Yu, X., Wang, Y., & Gaab, N. (2016). Lessons to be learned: How a comprehensive neurobiological framework of atypical reading development can inform educational practice. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

  • Payne, A. C., Whitehurst, G. J., & Angell, A. L. (1994). The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(3), 427–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child development, 816–833.

  • Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2015). Developmental dyslexia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 283–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L., Mencl, W. E., Austin, A. C., Sibley, D., et al. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain and Language, 125(2), 173–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., et al. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34(6), 479–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raizada, R. D., & Kishiyama, M. M. (2010). Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to levelling the playing field. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(3).

  • Raizada, R. D., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1392–1401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raschle, N. M., Zuk, J., & Gaab, N. (2012). Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 2156–2161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, T. L., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Abnormal fMRI connectivity in children with dyslexia during a phoneme task: before but not after treatment. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(4), 294–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, W., & Richter, M. (2003). The shape of the fMRI BOLD response in children and adults changes systematically with age. NeuroImage, 20(2), 1122–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Development, 82(4), 1058–1075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14(3), 245–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S., Dobrich, W., & Hager, M. (1991). Preschool literacy experience and later reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(8), 508–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaggar, B. L., & McCandliss, B. D. (2007). Development of neural systems for reading. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 475–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, S. A., Simpson, A. M., & Friend, M. (2011). A longitudinal assessment of the home literacy environment and early language. Infant and Child Development, 20(6), 409–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, E., Maurer, U., van der Mark, S., Bucher, K., Brem, S., Martin, E., et al. (2008). Impaired semantic processing during sentence reading in children with dyslexia: combined fMRI and ERP evidence. NeuroImage, 41(1), 153–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Sabers, D. (1980). CELF: Clinical evaluation of language functions: CE Merrill.

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(1), 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., Lefevre, J. A., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 96–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Blachman, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2004). Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55(9), 926–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52(2), 101–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., et al. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(5), 2636–2641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sideridis, G. D., Simos, P., Papanicolaou, A., & Fletcher, J. (2014). On the use of SEM for evaluating functional connectivity in the brain: sample size considerations. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74, 733–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (2006). Perspectives on dyslexia. Paediatrics & Child Health, 11(9), 581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s alpha. Psychometrika, 74, 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Castillo, E. M., Davis, R. N., et al. (2002). Brain activation profiles during the early stages of reading acquisition. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(3), 159–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. S., & Dixon, R. G. (1995). Literacy concepts of low-and middle-class four-year-olds entering preschool. The Journal of Educational Research, 88(4), 243–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 402–433.

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2001). The role of family and home in the literacy development of children from low‐income backgrounds. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2001(92), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, M., Wolff, J., Elison, J., Gu, H., Hazlett, H., Botteron, K., et al. (2015). Splenium development and early spoken language in human infants. Developmental Science.

  • Szaflarski, J. P., Holland, S. K., Schmithorst, V. J., & Byars, A. W. (2006). fMRI study of language lateralization in children and adults. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 202–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(5), 2860–2865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(2), 299–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomason, M. E., Burrows, B. E., Gabrieli, J. D., & Glover, G. H. (2005). Breath holding reveals differences in fMRI BOLD signal in children and adults. NeuroImage, 25(3), 824–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Tolvanen, A., Leskinen, E., Leppanen, P. H., et al. (2007). Modeling the early paths of phonological awareness and factors supporting its development in children with and without familial risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(2), 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 767–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Der Lely, H. (2000). Verb agreement and tense test (VATT). Available from the author at the centre for developmental language disorders and cognitive neuroscience. London: University College London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: new evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). CTOPP: Comprehensive test of phonological processing: Pro-ed.

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilke, M., Holland, S. K., Myseros, J. S., Schmithorst, V. J., & Ball, W. S., Jr. (2003). Functional magnetic resonance imaging in pediatrics. Neuropediatrics, 34(5), 225–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Schurz, M., Sturm, D., Richlan, F., Klackl, J., Kronbichler, M., et al. (2010). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: an fMRI study. Cortex, 46(10), 1284–1298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2005). Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests (RAN/RAS). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests, revised: American Guidance Service Circle Pines, MN.

  • World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoncheva, Y. N., Zevin, J. D., Maurer, U., & McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Auditory selective attention to speech modulates activity in the visual word form area. Cerebral Cortex, 20(3), 622–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research is supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development under grant 1R01HD065762-05.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nadine Gaab.

Additional information

Sara J. Powers and Yingying Wang contributed equally to this work.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Powers, S.J., Wang, Y., Beach, S.D. et al. Examining the relationship between home literacy environment and neural correlates of phonological processing in beginning readers with and without a familial risk for dyslexia: an fMRI study. Ann. of Dyslexia 66, 337–360 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0134-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0134-2

Keywords

Navigation