Skip to main content
Log in

Accumulation of experience in a vast number of cases: enactivism as a fit framework for the study of spatial reasoning in mathematics education

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

As we witness a push toward studying spatial reasoning as a principal component of mathematical competency and instruction in the twenty first century, we argue that enactivism, with its strong and explicit foci on the coupling of organism and environment, action as cognition, and sensory motor coordination provides an inclusive, expansive, apt, and fit framework. We illustrate the fit of enactivism as a theory of learning with data from an ongoing research project involving teachers and elementary-aged children’s engagement in the design and assembly of motorized robots. We offer that spatial reasoning with its considerations of physical context, the dynamics of a body moving through space, sensorimotor coordination, and cognition, appears different from other conceptual competencies in mathematics. Specifically, we argue that learner engagements with diverse types of informationally ‘dense’ visuo-spatial interfaces (e.g., blueprints, programming icons, blocks, maps), as in the research study, afford some of the necessary experiences with/in a vast number of cases described by Varela et al. (1991) that enable the development of other mathematical competencies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We use the current spelling—sensorimotor—which in our opinion also serves to signal the juxtaposition of sensory and motor coordination.

References

  • Barnett, T. (2013). What is spatial ability? wiseGEEK. Retrieved September 26, 2013, from http://www.wisegeek.org/what-is-spatial-ability.htm.

  • Begg, A. (2013). Interpreting enactivism for learning and teaching. Education Sciences and Society, 4(1). Retrieved January 15, 2014 from http://riviste.unimc.it/index.php/es_s/article/view/699.

  • Bergen, B. (2012). Louder than Words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L., & Coles, A. (2011). Developing expertise: how enactivism reframes mathematics teacher development. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 43(6–7), 861–873. doi:10.1007/s11858-011-0343-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, C. D., Moss, J., Sinclair, N., Whiteley, W., Okamoto, Y., McGarvey, L., et al. (2013). Early years spatial reasoning: Learning, teaching and research implications. Workshop Presented at the NCTM research presession: Linking research and practice, Denver, CO.

  • Carlson, A. G., Rowe, E., & Curby, T. W. (2013). Disentangling fine motor skills’ relations to academic achievement: the relative contributions of visual–spatial integration and visual–motor coordination. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(5), 514–533. doi:10.1080/00221325.2012.717122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B., Erkut, S., Ceder, I., & Young, J. (2008). Use of a storytelling context to improve girls’ and boys’ geometry skills in kindergarten. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 29–48.

  • Cummins, F. (2013). Towards an enactive account of action: speaking and joint speaking as exemplary domains. Adaptive Behavior, 21(3), 178–186. doi:10.1177/1059712313483144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B. (1996). Teaching mathematics: toward a sound alternative. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3(4), 357–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Jaegher, H., & Di Paulo, E. (2013). Enactivism is not interactionism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(345), 1–2. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00345.

  • Ernest, P. (2006). Reflections on theories of learning. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 38(1), 3–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gojak, L. (2012). Helping our students become mathematical thinkers. Presidential Address presented at the NCTM 2012 Annual Meeting, Philadelphia. Retrieved February 12, 2014, from http://www.nctm.org/conferences/content.aspx?id=33201.

  • Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017. doi:10.1037/a0020104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., Yoon, E. Y., Kumar, S., Lestou, V., Kitadono, K., Roberts, K. L., et al. (2010). The interaction of attention and action: from seeing action to acting on perception. British Journal of Psychology, 101(2), 185–206. doi:10.1348/000712609X458927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2013). Psychology unified: from folk psychology to radical enactivism. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), 174–178. doi:10.1037/a0032930.

  • Jansen, P., Schmelter, A., Quaiser-Pohl, C., Neuberger, S., & Heil, M. (2013). Mental rotation performance in primary school age children: are there gender differences in chronometric tests? Cognitive Development, 28(1), 51–62. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.08.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. NY: Farrar, Strauss & Giroux.

  • Kayhan, E. B. (2005). Investigation of high school students’ spatial ability. Middle East Technical University, Turkey. Retrieved September 25, 2013, from https://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12605771/index.pdf.

  • Keen, R., Carrico, R. L., Sylvia, M. R., & Berthier, N. E. (2003). How infants use perceptual information to guide action. Developmental Science, 6(2), 221–231. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: how the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479–1498. doi:10.2307/1130467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maheux, J. F., & Proulx, J. (2015). Doing|mathematics: Analysing data with/in an enactivist-inspired approach. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 47(2).

  • Mgombelo, J., & Reid, D. A. (2015). Key concepts in enactivist theory and methodology. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 47(2).

  • Newcombe, N. S. (2010). Picture this: increasing math and science learning by improving spatial thinking. American Educator, 34(2), 29–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pouw, W. T. J. L., van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2014). An embedded and embodied cognition review of instructional manipulatives. Education Psychology Review, 26, 51–72. doi:10.1007/s10648-014-9255-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Presmeg, N. (2014). Contemplating visualisation as an epistemological learning tool in mathematics. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(1), 151–157. doi:10.1007/s11858-013-0561-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proulx, J. (2013). Mental mathematics, emergence of strategies, and the enactivist theory of cognition. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 84(3), 309–328. doi:10.1007/s10649-013-9480-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rivera, F. D., Steinbring, H., & Arcavi, A. (2014). Visualisation as an epistemological learning tool: an introduction. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(1), 1–2. doi:10.1007/s11858-013-0552-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, S. K. (2008). Perceptually guided action: a step in the right direction. Current Biology, 18(1), R36–R37. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, S. D. (2013). Children’s spatial skills seen as key to math learning. Education Week, 32(31). Retrieved January 8, 2014, from http://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2013/05/15/31learning.h32.html.

  • Tepylo, D. (2013). Spatial reasoning: considerations for mathematics educators. Toronto: University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1999). Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford University Press.

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villalobos, M. (2013). Enactive cognitive science: revisionism or revolution? Adaptive Behaviour, 21(3), 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1995). Radical constructivism: a way of knowing and learning. Washington, D.C.: Falmer Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Imperial Oil Science Engineering and Technology (IOSTEM) Education Initiative. We thank the teachers and children who participated in the IOSTEM Academy. We are also grateful to Michael Poscente for designing and coaching the Lego™ robotics task and Dr. Shalini Khan for editorial and proofreading assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven Khan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Khan, S., Francis, K. & Davis, B. Accumulation of experience in a vast number of cases: enactivism as a fit framework for the study of spatial reasoning in mathematics education. ZDM Mathematics Education 47, 269–279 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-014-0623-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-014-0623-x

Keywords

Navigation