Skip to main content
Log in

Using Reflectance Ratios to Study Animal Coloration

  • Tools and Techniques
  • Published:
Evolutionary Biology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 22 September 2015

Abstract

Reflectance spectrophotometry is used to quantify animal coloration and compare it across individuals, populations or species. While principles of colour perception, pigment-based colour production, and some structural-based colour production, all conform to reflectance differences on a ratio scale, reflectance is usually measured on a linear scale. Processing reflectance spectra on a linear scale distorts colour information (e.g. averaging linear spectra overestimates mean brightness), and computing receiver-independent colour metrics using linear reflectance can bias results (e.g. it underestimates differences in saturation and brightness among dark colours). Working with reflectance spectra on a ratio scale obviates those issues and improves several aspects of colour analysis, from better visualization of reflectance spectra and control of measurement error, to better colour metrics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akre, K. L., & Johnsen, S. (2014). Psychophysics and the evolution of behaviour. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 29, 291–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, S., & Prager, M. (2006). Quantifying colors. In G. E. Hill & K. J. McGraw (Eds.), Bird coloration, vol. 1, mechanisms and measurements (pp. 41–89). Boston: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, P. D. (1981). Visual contrast thresholds in the cod Gadus morhua L. Journal of Fish Biology, 19, 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Billmeyer, F. W., & Saltzman, M. (1981). Principles of colour technology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chappelle, E. W., Kim, M. S., & McMurtrey, J. E. (1992). Ratio analysis of reflectance spectra RARS: An algorithm for the remote estimation of the concentrations of chlorophyll a, chlorophyll b, and carotenoids in soybean leaves. Remote Sensing of Environment, 39, 239–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Datt, B. (1999). A new reflectance index for remote sensing of chlorophyll content in higher plants: Tests using Eucalyptus leaves. Journal of Plant Physiology, 154, 30–36.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2003). The neural basis of the Weber-Fechner law: A logarithmic mental number line. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 145–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A., & Mielke, P. W. J. (2005). Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 86, 405–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. R., Hinks, A. E., Wilkin, T. A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2010). Age, sex and beauty: Methodological dependence of age- and sex-dichromatism in the great tit Parus major. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 101, 777–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gingerich, P. D. (2000). Arithmetic or geometric normality of biological variation: An empirical test of theory. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 204, 201–221.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, B. E. (2010). Sensation and Perception (8th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griebel, U., & Schmid, A. (1999). Brightness discrimination ability in the west indian manatee (Trichechus manatus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 200, 1587–1592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houle, D., Pélabon, C., Wagner, G. P., & Hansen, T. F. (2011). Measurement and meaning in biology. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 86, 3–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S. (2008). Structural colors in the realm of nature. Singapore: World Scientific.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S., & Yoshioka, S. (2005). Structural colors in nature: The role of regularity and irregularity in the structure. ChemPhysChem, 6, 1442–1459.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, O., Karlsson, S., & Kelber, A. (2013). Brightness discrimination in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). PLoS One, 8, e54650.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maia, R., Eliason, C. M., Bitton, P.-P., Doucet, S. M., & Shawkey, M. D. (2013). Pavo: An R package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral data. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4, 906–913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomerie, R. (2006). Analyzing colors. In G. E. Hill & K. J. McGraw (Eds.), Bird coloration, vol. 1, mechanisms and measurements (pp. 90–147). Boston: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicol, J. A. C. (1989). The eyes of fishes. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholtyssek, C., Kelber, A., & Dehnhardt, G. (2008). Brightness discrimination in the harbor seal (Phoca vitulina). Vision Research, 48, 96–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siddiqi, A., Cronin, T. W., Loew, E. R., Vorobyev, M., & Summers, K. (2004). Interspecific and intraspecific views of color signals in the strawberry poison frog Dendrobates pumilio. Journal of Experimental Biology, 207, 2471–2485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sims, D. A., & Gamon, J. A. (2002). Relationships between leaf pigment content and spectral reflectance across a wide range of species, leaf structures and developmental stages. Remote Sensing of Environment, 81, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard, M. C., & Prum, R. O. (2008). Evolution of avian plumage color in a tetrahedral color space: A phylogenetic analysis of new world buntings. American Naturalist, 171, 755–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taysom, A. J., Stuart-Fox, D., & Cardoso, G. C. (2010). The contribution of structural-, psittacofulvin- and melanin-based colouration to sexual dichromatism in Australasian parrots. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24, 303–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T. D., Marshall, N., & Cuthill, I. (1998). Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 183, 621–633.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Reviewer suggestions much improved the original manuscript. This article was supported by fellowship SFRH/BPD/46873/2008 and grant PTDC/BIA-BEC/098414/2008 from the Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gonçalo C. Cardoso.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Chromatic and achromatic contrasts (CC and AC, respectively) between pairs of adjacent spectra in Figs. 1 and 2 were calculated, in units of just noticeable differences, using the visual model of Vorobyev et al. (1998; see also Siddiqi et al. 2004) as implemented in the R package pavo (v0.5-1, functions vismodel and coldist ; Maia et al. 2013). CC is computed using quantum catches from single-cones, and AC using quantum catches from double-cones. Visual model settings were as follow: average UV avian sensitivity, regular daylight illumination, von Kries correction, absolute quantum catches according to Fechner law, and the remaining options set to default (contrasts are identical whether modelling with or without von Kries correction, applying Fechner law or not).

To illustrate how we would approximately perceive hue and saturation of the spectra in Figs. 1 and 2, we used the function spec2rgb in pavo . The resulting coordinates in RGB (red–green–blue) colour space were converted to coordinates in the HSL (hue–saturation–lightness) colour space, and the lightness coordinates set to an identical, arbitrary value to illustrate chromatic differences while holding brightness constant.

To illustrate how we would approximately perceive colour brightness of the spectra in Figs. 1 and 2, we measured emitted light of a greyscale, from black to white at intervals of 5 % lightness, on a computer monitor with an Ocean Optics USB4000 spectrophotometer, calibrating emission spectra with black and white measurements on the same monitor. We then chose the tone of grey whose mean log light emission across the human-visible wavelengths (ca. 400–700 nm) best matched those of each spectrum in Figs. 1 and 2.

Appendix 2

We used the data of Taysom et al. (2010) on pigment-based red to yellow colour of Australasian parrots, but here flattened reflectance values lower than 1 % in the original spectra to 1 %, and used geometric rather than arithmetic means to average across the spectra of each individual and then across the different individuals of each species and sex (see the section ‘Recommendations’ in the main text). We calculated the achromatic contrast between the mean spectrum of males and females for each species, using the visual models of Vorobyev et al. (1998) as implemented in pavo (Maia et al. 2013) with the same settings as described before. For comparison with these achromatic contrasts, we computed absolute values of the difference between the sexes in receiver-independent metrics of brightness: mean reflectance on a linear (% reflectance) or on a ratio (log10 % reflectance) scale of reflectance, across the bird-visible frequency range (ca. 300–700 nm). Mean brightness for each species (horizontal axes in Fig. 3) was computed as the mean of male and female log10 % reflectance across their visible frequency range.

Appendix 3

We used 8911 reflectance spectrophotometry measurements of plumage colours, made on museum skins of 135 species of finches in the family Estrildidae. For most species male and female specimens were measured, usually three specimens per species/sex, and multiple measurements of colour were made that include all the main body parts. This dataset will be described in detail elsewhere.

Here we applied visual models to estimate colour saturation of these spectra as the distance to the achromatic centre of an avian tetrahedral colour space (Endler and Mielke 2005; Stoddard and Prum 2008). Reflectance values lower than 1 % were flattened to 1 %, and spectra were then analysed using the avian visual model of Vorobyev et al. (1998) as implemented in pavo (functions vismodel and tcs ; Maia et al. 2013). Visual model settings were as follow: average UV avian sensitivity, regular daylight illumination, von Kries correction, relative quantum catches according to Fechner law (i.e. log-transforming quantum catches, for the x-axis of Fig. 4) or relative quantum catches without Fechner law (i.e. not log-transforming, for the y-axis of Fig. 4), and the remaining options set to default.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cardoso, G.C., Gomes, A.C.R. Using Reflectance Ratios to Study Animal Coloration. Evol Biol 42, 387–394 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-015-9328-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-015-9328-5

Keywords

Navigation