Skip to main content
Log in

Home literacy as a special language environment to prepare children for school

  • Schwerpunkt: Familienbildung
  • Published:
Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

The present study examined the relationship between language and literacy activities at home and the emergence of so called academic language in a sample of 68 four-year-old Dutch children. The focus was on children’s understanding and production of narrative texts, seen as a common age-appropriate pre-academic text genre. Home language and literacy was measured with a short questionnaire, administered in personal interviews with the mothers. Other measures were the family’s socioeconomic status (SES), children’s receptive vocabulary and working memory. Children’s productive language in narrative text (re)telling already revealed many features of academic language. Furthermore, moderate to strong correlations were found between children’s emergent academic language and the language and literacy activities at home. Multiple regression analyses indicated that, in addition to working memory, home talking and reading predicted children’s vocabulary, and especially children’s text comprehension and text (re)telling. Finally, main and interaction-effects of working memory and home language and literacy on emergent academic language were examined. Although the results were not conclusive regarding the expected moderator effect of working memory, they revealed that children with both low working memory capacity and low academic language input were most disadvantaged.

Zusammenfassung

Elterliche Sprachkompetenz als besondere Sprachumgebung zur Heranführung des Kindes an die Schule

Die vorliegende Studie untersuchte den Zusammenhang zwischen der im Elternhaus entwickelten Sprach-bzw. Lesepraxis und der Entwicklung der so genannten akademischen Sprachkompetenz, in Deutschland auch als ‚bildungssprachliche Kompetenz’ bezeichnet. Hierzu wurden in den Niederlanden insgesamt 68 Kinder im Alter von 4 Jahren untersucht. Es wurde insbesondere untersucht, inwieweit die Kinder in der Lage sind, Geschichten zu verstehen und zu produzieren. Geschichten werden im Rahmen der Studie als altersgemäße vorakademische Texte aufgefasst. Der Familiensprachgebrauch und die literarische Praxis wurden anhand eines kurzen Fragebogens und in Interviews mit den Müttern erhoben. Zusätzlich wurden der sozioökonomische Status der Familie sowie der passive Wortschatz der Kinder und deren Merkfähigkeit unter Arbeitsbedingungen (working memory) herangezogen.

Die Nacherzählungen der Kinder wiesen sprachlich bereits viele Merkmale der so genannten akademischen Sprache auf. Außerdem zeigte sich ein mittlerer bis starker Zusammenhang zwischen der sich entwickelnden Sprachfähigkeit der Kinder (akademische Sprache) und den sprachlichen und literarischen Praktiken der Familie. Multiple Regressionsanalysen haben gezeigt, dass — neben dem Arbeitsgedächtnis (working memory) der Kinder insbesondere die Qualität der Sprache innerhalb der Familie und das gemeinsame Lesen im Elternhaus positiven Einfluss auf den Wortschatz und das Textverständnis der Kinder hatten. Außerdem wurde die Fähigkeit, Geschichten nachzuerzählen dadurch besonders gefördert.

Schließlich wurde die Wirkung des Arbeitsgedächtnisses und der mitgebrachten Sprachkompetenz auf die Entwicklung einer akademischen Sprache überprüft. Obwohl die Resultate den erwarteten Effekt nicht vollends bestätigen konnten, zeigte sich, dass Kinder mit geringer Merkfähigkeit bei gleichzeitig geringem Kontakt mit akademischer Sprachpraxis im Elternhaus in besonderer Weise benachteiligt waren.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, A.-M. /Gathercole, S. E. (2000): Limitations of working memory: Implications for language development. In: International Journal of Communication Disorders, Vol. 35(1), pp. 95–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P. /Gathercole, S. E. /Pickering, S. J. (2006): Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? In: Child Development, Vol. 77(6), pp. 1698–1716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. B. /Stokes, S. J. (1984): Social and institutional influences on the development and practice of literacy. In: Goelman, H. /Oberg, A. /Smith, F. (Eds.)): Awakening to literacy. — London, pp. 24–37.

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2003): Working memory and language: An overview. In: International Journal of Communication Disorders, Vol. 36, pp. 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker et al. 2001 = Baker, L. /Mackler, K. /Sonnenschein, S. /Serpell, R. (2001): Parents’ interactions with their first-grade children during storybook reading and relations with subsequent home reading activity and reading achievement. In: Journal of School Psychology, Vol. 39(5), pp. 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beals, D. E. (1997): Sources of support for learning words in conversation: Evidence from mealtimes. In: Journal of Child Language, Vol. 24, pp. 673–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beals, D. E. (2001) Eating and reading: Links between family conversations with preschoolers and later language literacy. — Baltimore.

  • Bernstein, B. (1977): Social class, language and socialization. In: Karabel, J. /Halsley, A. H. (Eds.): Power and ideology in education. — New York, pp. 473–486.

  • Blank, M. /Rose, S. A. /Berlin, L. J. (1978): The language of learning: The preschool years. — New York.

  • Blok et al. 2005 = Blok, H. /Fukkink, R. G. /Gebhardt, E. C. /Leseman, P. P. M. (2005): The relevance of delivery mode and other program characteristics for the effectiveness of early childhood intervention with disadvantaged children. In: International Journal of Behavioral Development, Vol. 29, pp. 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2000): How children learn the meaning of words. — Cambridge/London.

  • Bot-De Vries, A. (2006): What’s in a name? — Leiden (unpublished doctoral thesis).

  • Bourdieu, P. /Passeron, J. C. (1977): Reproduction in education, society and culture. — London.

  • Bus, A. G. /Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. /Pellegrini, A. D. (1995): Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read. A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. In: Review of Educational Research, Vol. 65, pp. 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H. /Corwyn, R. F. (2002): Socioeconomic status and child development. In: Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 53, pp. 371–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gun, J. /Markman, L. B. (2005): The contribution of parenting to ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness. In: The Future of Children, Vol. 15(1), pp. 139–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crain-Thoreson, C. /Dahlin, M. P. /Powell, T.A. (2001): Parent-child interaction in three conversational contexts: Variations in style and strategy. In: New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Vol. 92, pp. 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DASH (2006): Coding protocol for functional linguistic analysis. — Amsterdam.

  • De Jong, P. F. /Leseman, P. P. M. (2001): Lasting effects of home literacy on reading achievement in school. In: Journal of School Psychology, Vol. 39(5), pp. 389–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A. /Shaw, D. /Bell, M. (2000): Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. In: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 54, pp. 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, J.C. /Barron, R.W. /Brunello, M. (2000): Direct and mediated influences of home literacy and literacy interest on prereaders’ oral vocabulary and written language skill. In: Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 92(3), pp. 466–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (2001): The social context of cognitive development. — New York.

  • Goff, D. A. /Pratt, C. /Ong, B. (2005): The relations between children’s reading comprehension, working memory, language skills and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. In: Reading and Writing, Vol. 18, pp. 583–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haden, C.A. /Haine, R.A. /Fivush, R. (1997): Developing narrative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. In: Developmental Psychology, Vol. 33(2), pp. 295–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1994): An introduction to functional grammar (Second Edition). — London.

  • Hammett, L. A. / van Kleeck, A. /Huberty, C. J. (2003): Patterns of parents’ extratextual interactions with preschool children: a cluster analysis study. In: Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 38(4), pp. 442–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. B. (1983): Ways with words. — Cambridge, Massachusetts.

  • Hoff, E. (2006): How social contexts support and shape language development. In: Developmental Review, Vol. 26, pp. 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. /Naigles, L. (2002): How children use input to acquire a lexicon. In: Child Development, Vol. 73(2), pp. 418–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher et al. 2002 = Huttenlocher, J. /Vasilyeva, M. /Cymerman, E. /Levine, S. (2002): Language input and child syntax. In: Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 45, pp. 337–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karrass, J. /Braungart-Rieker, J.M. (2005): Effects of shared parent-infant book reading on early language acquisition. In: Applied Developmental Psychology, Vol. 26, pp. 133–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (2004): The construction-integration model of text comprehension and its implications for instruction. In: Ruddell, R. B. /Unrau, N. J. (Eds.): Theoretical models and processes of reading (5th edition). — Newark, pp. 1270–1328.

  • Landry, S. H. /Smith, K. E. /Swank, P. R. (2003): The importance of parenting during early childhood for school-age development. In: Developmental Neuropsychology, Vol. 24(2 and 3), pp. 559–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leseman, P. P. M. / de Jong, P. F. (1998): Home literacy: opportunity, instruction, cooperation, and socialemotional quality predicting early reading achievement. In: Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 33(3), pp. 294–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leseman, P. P. M. /De Jong, P. F. (2001): How important is home literacy for acquiring literacy in school? In Verhoeven, L. /Snow, C. E. (Eds.): Literacy and motivation: reading engagement in individuals and groups. — Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 71–93.

  • Leseman, P. P. M. /De Jong, P. F. (2004): Förderung der Sprach-und Präliteralitätsentwicklung in der Familie und Vorschule. In: Faust, G. /Götz, M. /Hacker, H./ H.-G. Rossbach (Hrsg.): Anschlussfähige Bildungsprozesse im Elementar-und Primarbereich. — Bad Heilbrunn, S. 168–189.

  • Leseman, P. P. M. /Van Tuijl, C. (2006): Cultural diversity in early literacy development. In: Neuman, S. B. /Dickinson, D. K. (Eds.): Handbook of early literacy research. — Vol. 2 — New York, pp. 211–228.

  • Matthews et al. 2005 = Matthews, D. /Lieven, E. V. M. /Theakston, A. L. /Tomasello, M. (2005): The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. In: Cognitive Development, Vol. 20(1), pp. 121–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1991): Literacy as metalinguistic activity. In: Olson, D. R. /Torrance, N. (Eds.): Literacy and orality. — Cambridge, England, pp. 251–270.

  • Pelletier, J. /Astington, J. W. (2004): Action, consciousness and theory of mind: Children’s ability to coordinate story characters’ actions and thoughts. In: Early Education and Development, Vol. 15(1), pp. 5–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Purcell-Gates, V. (1996): Stories, coupons, and the TV guide: Relationships between home literacy experiences and emergent literacy knowledge. In: Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 31, pp. 406–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. /Tolchinsky, L. (2002): Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. In: Journal of Child Language, Vol. 29, pp. 417–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raviv, T. /Kessenich, M. /Morrison, F. J. (2004): A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: the role of parenting factors. In: Early Childhood Research Quarterly, Vol. 19, pp. 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schleppegrell, M. (2004): The language of schooling. — Mahwah.

  • Schneider, W. /Roth, E. /Ennemoser, M. (2000): Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. In: Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 92(2), pp. 284–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. /Lefevre, J. (2002): Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. In: Child Development, Vol. 73, pp. 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E. /Stinson, E. T. /Kim, M. (1993): Socialization of cognition: The distancing model. In: Wozniak, R. H. /Fischer, K. W. (Eds.): Development in context. Acting and thinking in specific environments. — New York, pp. 211–224.

  • Snow, C. E. (1991): The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy development. In: Journal of Research in Childhood Education, Vol. 6(1), pp. 5–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1999): Facilitating language development promotes literacy learning. In: Eldering, L. /Leseman, P. (Eds.): Effective early education: Cross-cultural perspectives. — New York, pp. 141–161.

  • Snow, C. E. /Kurland, B. (1996): Sticking to the point: Talk about magnets as a preparation for literacy. In: Hicks, D. (Ed.): Child discourse and social learning: An interdisciplinary perspective. — New York, pp. 189–220.

  • Storch, S. A. /Whitehurst, G. J. (2002): Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. In: Developmental Psychology, Vol. 38(6), pp. 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulzby, E. (1986): Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language organization in the young child. In: Teale, W. H. /Sulzby, E. (Eds.): Emergent literacy: writing and reading. — Norwood, pp. 50–89.

  • Sylva et al. 2004 = Sylva, K. /Melhuish, E. /Sammons, P. /Siraj-Blatchford, I. /Taggart, B. (2004): The Effective Provision of Pre-school Education (EPPE) project: Findings from Pre-school to end of Key Stage 1. — London.

  • Theakston, A. L. /Lieven, E. V. M. /Tomashello, M. (2003): The role of input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. In: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Vol. 46(4), pp. 863–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003): Constructing a language. — Cambridge, Massachusetts.

  • Van Kleeck et al. 1997 = Van Kleeck, A. /Gillam, R. /Hamilton, L. /McGrath, C. (1997): The relationship between middle-class parents’ book sharing discussion and their preschoolers’ abstract language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Vol. 40, pp. 1261–1271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhallen, M. /Schoonen, R. (1998): Lexical knowledge in L1 and L2 of 3rd and 5th graders. In: Applied Linguistics, Vol. 19(4), pp. 452–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeer, A. (2001): Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to acquisition and frequency of input. In: Applied Psycholinguistics, Vol. 22, pp. 217–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vlaardingerbroek et al. 2007 = Vlaardingerbroek, G. /Leseman, P. P. M. / van Luit, J. E. H. /Messer, M. H. /Scheele, A. F. (2007): Visuo-spatial working memory in young children’s language comprehension. — Utrecht (paper submitted for publication).

  • Weizman, Z. O. /Snow, C. E. (2001): Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. In: Developmental Psychology, Vol. 37(2), pp. 265–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J. /Lonigan, C. J. (1998): Child development and emergent literacy. In: Child Development, Vol. 69(3), pp. 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul P. M. Leseman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Leseman, P.P.M., Scheele, A.F., Mayo, A.Y. et al. Home literacy as a special language environment to prepare children for school. ZfE 10, 334–355 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-007-0040-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-007-0040-9

Keywords

Schlüsselwörter

Navigation