Skip to main content
Log in

Plural exponence in the Nez Perce DP: a DM analysis

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper analyzes two patterns of number marking in the DP in Nez Perce (Sahaptian) within the framework of Distributed Morphology. The first involves under-realization of plural on nouns. Number has classically been understood as a feature inherent to nouns, rather than to adjectives that modify them. In Nez Perce, however, only a small set of nouns show number morphology, whereas number morphology is highly productive on adjectival modifiers. Adjectives in fact may realize the plural more than once per word—an instance of multiple exponence. I show that the puzzle of under-realization for nouns can be solved through conditioned allomorphy, providing new evidence for the presence of gender features on n (Lowenstamm 2007; Kramer 2014). The puzzle of multiple exponence (over-realization) for adjectives can be solved through Local Dislocation (Embick and Noyer 2001) combined with very late insertion of reduplicative content (Haugen 2008, 2011), demonstrating one way the DM architecture may produce multiple exponence without recourse to dedicated mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Nez Perce lacks vowel-initial words, so words in this class always begin with a non-glottal consonant.

  2. Lexical data in this paper draws primarily from the extensive Nez Perce dictionary, Aoki (1994); references to this work are given as [An], where n is a page number. All data not credited to a published source comes from fieldwork conducted on the Nez Perce reservation in Lapwai, ID, 2006–2015, with four native speaker consultants. In some cases, data from fieldnotes is given the tag [fn]. Transcriptions are based on the Aoki 1994 system. Note that e = IPA æ, y = IPA j,  = IPA χ, c = IPA ts, and that main stress is indicated with an acute accent. Transcriptions here depart from Aoki in marking vowel length with digraphs, and glottalization with an apostrophe just after the glottalized consonant. They depart from transcriptions used in Deal (2010a,b) and following work in marking the glottal stop with ʔ instead of an apostrophe.

  3. For discussion and references, see Sect. 2.

  4. See for instance Corbett’s (2000, p. 2) fifth “reasonable but incorrect assumption” about number: “Number is a nominal category.” The Nez Perce pattern in (6)–(7) is also relevant to the second reasonable but incorrect assumption Corbett discusses: “all relevant items (nouns, for instance) will mark number.”

  5. Note that Nez Perce generally does not tolerate post-consonantal glottal consonants. Singular form ʔilpʔilp ‘red’ (8d) is exceptional in this regard.

  6. See Deal (2015) for paradigms and discussion.

  7. This form is used in pedagogical materials circulated by the Nez Perce tribe. Aoki (1994) reports that there is no specialized human form for this numeral.

  8. The derivation of this form is presumably ʔoymata + wa > ʔoymatoo. Coalescence of awa to oo is frequent in Nez Perce; see Crook (1999, Chap. 3).

  9. Aoki records both this form and initial-stress variant píilepuʔ.

  10. This generalization may not be fully universal; see Vázquez Rojas Maldonado (2012) and Dalrymple and Mofu (2012) for discussion.

  11. This list is drawn from unpublished work by Harold Crook.

  12. Aoki (1994) records a small number of exceptions to this pattern: (mi)méex̂sem ‘mountain(s)’ [A437], (pi)póhol ‘ravine(s)’ [A555], (si)sáqan ‘canyon(s)’ [A623], (he)ʔískit ‘road(s)’ [A1078]. My consultants report a varying degree of familiarity with the plurals of these words, but are not comfortable using them in sentences. They are comfortable with plural forms only for human-class nouns.

  13. The concord process itself may be understand in a variety of ways; I leave this question open here. See Baker (2008), Carstens (2001), and Toosarvandani and van Urk (2014) for discussion of Agree-based concord, and Norris (2012, 2014) for discussion of concord via morphological feature copying.

  14. The appearance of /n/ is conditioned by the class of the root. Root class is syntactically determined in Nez Perce (Deal and Wolf 2015).

  15. On the case system of Nez Perce, see Deal (2010a, 2010b).

  16. No examples of this type have been located in Aoki and Walker (1989), the source of examples (45) above, and speakers generally reject these forms in elicitation.

  17. In view of the separate exponence of number and case in examples like (47), I assume that these features are fissioned at the point of Vocabulary Insertion. On fission, see Noyer (1992), Halle (2000).

  18. CVC words sis ‘navel’, pis ‘drizzle’ and tit ‘tooth’ are potential exceptions. Harold Crook (p.c.) reports final consonant lengthening in these forms, however, in keeping with the hypothesis that true CVC words are sub-minimal. I am not aware of any potential CV content words.

  19. Recall that this structure represents only constituency, not linear order; it is fully equivalent to the structures discussed in Sect. 3, where Agr was displayed to the right of its sister.

  20. This view may be contrasted with rule-based phonological approaches advocated by Embick 2010 and (with particular emphasis on reduplication) by Raimy 2000a, 2000b and Frampton 2009.

  21. The major alternative is to treat Agr as an adjunct to the complex a head. If this is so, then Lowering ipso facto cannot bring Agr into the desired configuration (61), as Agr has no complement that it may be lowered to.

References

  • Adger, D., Bejar, S., & Harbour, D. (2003). Directionality of allomorphy: a reply to Carstairs-McCarthy. Transactions of the Philological Society, 101, 109–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. R. (2001). On some issues in morphological exponence. In Yearbook of morphology (pp. 1–17).

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, H. (1963). Reduplication in Nez Perce. International Journal of American Linguistics, 29, 42–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, H. (1970). Nez Perce grammar. University of California publications in linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, H. (1994). Nez Perce dictionary. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, H., & Walker, D. (1989). Nez Perce oral narratives. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. C. (2008). The syntax of agreement and concord. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bakovic, E. (2000). Harmony, dominance and control. Doctoral Dissertation, Rutgers University.

  • Bobaljik, J. (2000). The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy. In K. K. Grohmann & C. Struijke (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1999 Maryland Mayfest on Morphology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2005). In name only. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bye, P., & Svenonius, P. (2012). Exponence, phonology and non-concatenative morphology. In J. Trommer (Ed.), The morphology and phonology of exponence (pp. 427–495). London: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Caballero, G., & Harris, A. C. (2012). A working typology of multiple exponence. In F. Kiefer, M. Ladányi, & P. Siptár (Eds.), Current issues in morphological theory: (ir)regularity, analogy and frequency. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstens, V. (2001). Multiple agreement and case deletion: against phi-(in)completeness. Syntax, 4, 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (1998). Reference to kinds across languages. Natural Language Semantics, 6, 339–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, G. G. (2000). Number. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Crook, H. D. (1999). The phonology and morphology of Nez Perce stress. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles.

  • Dalrymple, M., & Mofu, S. (2012). Plural semantics, reduplication and numeral modification in Indonesian. Journal of Semantics, 29, 229–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deal, A. R. (2010a). Ergative case and the transitive subject: a view from Nez Perce. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 28, 73–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deal, A. R. (2010b). Topics in the Nez Perce verb. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst.

  • Deal, A. R. (2015). A note on Nez Perce verb agreement, with sample paradigms. In N. Weber, E. Guntly, Z. Lam, & S. Chen (Eds.), Proceedings of the international conference on Salish and neighbouring languages 50 (pp. 389–413). Vancouver: UBCWPL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deal, A. R., & Wolf, M. (2015, in press). Outwards-sensitive phonologically-conditioned allomorphy in Nez Perce. In V. Gribanova & S. Shih (Eds.), The morphosyntax-phonology connection. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D. (2010). Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Noyer, R. (2001). Movement operations after syntax. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 555–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frampton, J. (2009). Distributed reduplication. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gribanova, V. (2010). Composition and locality: the morphosyntax and phonology of the Russian verbal complex. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.

  • Gribanova, V., & Harizanov, B. (2015, in press). Locality and directionality in inward-sensitive allomorphy: Russian and Bulgarian. In V. Gribanova & S. Shih (Eds.), The morphosyntax-phonology connection. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, B., & Hall, R. H. R. (1977). Nez Perce vowel harmony: an Africanist explanation and some theoretical questions. In R. M. Vago (Ed.), Issues in vowel harmony (pp. 201–236). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M. (2000). Distributed morphology: impoverishment and fission. In J. Lecarme, J. Lowenstamm, & U. Shlonsky (Eds.), Research in Afroasiatic grammar (pp. 125–149). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Harizanov, B., & Gribanova, V. (2011). The role of morphological and phonological factors in Bulgarian allomorph selection. In Morphology at Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz: Linguistics Research Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, H. (1994). Hug a tree: deriving the morphosyntactic feature hierarchy. In MIT working papers in linguistics 21: papers on phonology and morphology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, H., & Ritter, E. (2002). Person and number in pronouns: a feature-geometric analysis. Language, 78, 482–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, A. C. (2009). Exuberant exponence in Batsbi. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 27, 267–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haugen, J. D. (2008). Morphology at the interfaces: reduplication and noun incorporation in Uto-Aztecan. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haugen, J. D. (2011). Reduplication in distributed morphology. In Coyote papers (Vol. 18).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobsen, W. (1968). On the history of Nez Perce vowel harmony. Language, 44, 819–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Li. (2012). Nominal arguments and language variation. Doctoral Dissertation, Harvard.

  • Kramer, R. (2014). Gender in Amharic: a morphosyntactic approach to natural and grammatical gender. Language Sciences, 43, 102–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (2007). On little n, √, and types of nouns. In J. Hartmann, V. Hegedus, & H. van Riemsdjik (Eds.), The sounds of silence: empty elements in syntax and phonology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackenzie, S., & Dresher, B. E. (2004). Contrast and phonological activity in the Nez Perce vowel system. In P. Nowak et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of BLS 29 (pp. 283–294). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mascaró, J. (1996). External allomorphy as emergence of the unmarked. In J. Durand & B. Laks (Eds.), Current trends in phonology: models and methods (pp. 473–483). Salford: European Studies Research Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. J., & Prince, A. (1993). Prosodic morphology: Constraint interaction and satisfaction. Technical report, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.

  • Mester, R. A. (1994). The quantitative trochee in Latin. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 12, 1–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, G. (2007). Extended exponence by enrichment: argument encoding in German, Archi, and Timucua. In U. Penn working papers in linguistics series 13 (pp. 253–266).

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, M. (2012). Towards an analysis of concord (in Icelandic). In J. Choi et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL 29 (pp. 205–213). Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, M. (2014). A theory of nominal concord. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.

  • Noyer, R. (1992). Features, positions and affixes in autonomous morphological structure. Doctoral Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Raimy, E. (2000a). The phonology and morphology of reduplication. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Raimy, E. (2000b). Remarks on backcopying. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 541–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigsby, B., & Silverstein, M. (1969). Nez Perce vowels and Proto-Sahaptian vowel harmony. Language, 45, 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, E. (1991). Two functional categories in the noun phrase: evidence from Modern Hebrew. In S. Anderson & S. D. Rothstein (Eds.), Syntax and semantics 25 (pp. 37–62).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, E. (1993). Where’s gender? Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 795–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rude, N. (1985). Studies in Nez Perce grammar and discourse. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Oregon.

  • Toosarvandani, M., & van Urk, C. (2014). The syntax of nominal concord: what ezafe in Zazaki shows us. In H.-L. Huang, E. Poole, & A. Rsyling (Eds.), Proceedings of NELS 43 (Vol. 2, pp. 209–220). Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • T’sou, B. K. (1976). The structure of nominal classifier systems. In Oceanic linguistics special publications (Vol. 13, pp. 1215–1247).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vázquez Rojas Maldonado, V. (2012). The syntax and semantics of Purépecha noun phrases and the mass/count distinction. Doctoral Dissertation, New York University.

  • Zimmer, K. (1967). A note on vowel harmony. International Journal of American Linguistics, 33, 166–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, A. M. (1971). More on Nez Perce: an alternative analysis. International Journal of American Linguistics, 37, 122–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Rose Deal.

Additional information

I’d like to thank audience members at Allomorphy: its logic and limitations, along with Harold Crook, Kathryn Pruitt, Maziar Toosarvandani, and two anonymous Morphology reviewers. A special thanks is due to Bessie Scott and Florene Davis for teaching me so much about their beautiful Nez Perce language over the years. And thanks to Noam Faust for organizing the Jerusalem workshop that eventually led to this paper being written.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Deal, A.R. Plural exponence in the Nez Perce DP: a DM analysis. Morphology 26, 313–339 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-015-9277-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-015-9277-9

Keywords

Navigation